Skip to main content
Log in

An experimental analysis of ongoing verbal behavior: Reinforcement, verbal operants, and superstitious behavior

  • Published:
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Four adult humans were asked to asked to “find” and talk about a particular topic to a person in an adjoining room, and were instructed that they would hear a short beep (the only form of reply from the other person) when they were talking about the topic, or were “close” to the topic. In Session 1, the experimenter in the adjoining room presented the beeps in the manner of shaping, or the differential reinforcement of successive approximations, “toward” the designated topic. In Session 2, the same conditions were in effect but the experimenter was unable to hear the subject and the beeps were presented noncontingently in a way that roughly matched the frequency and distribution of presentations in Session 1. In Session 3, shaping conditions were again in effect but with a different topic than that designated for Session 1. Audio recordings were transcribed in a way that was designed to show the progress of shaping over time. These and additional forms of supporting data and accompanying rationale are presented and discussed in detail. Issues raised by the methodology and results of the experiment include the nature of the verbal operant, superstitious verbal behavior, and a variety of methodological issues relevant to the experimental analysis of ongoing or continuous verbal behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Azrin, N., Holz, W., Ulrich, R., & Goldiamond, I. (1961). The control of the content of conversation through reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 25–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Braam, S. J., & Sundberg, M. L. (1991). The effect of specific versus nonspecific reinforcement on verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 19–28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1992). Learning (3rd Ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cordova, J. V., & Koerner, K. (1993). Persuation criteria in research and practice: Gathering more meaningful psychotherapy data. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 317–330.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Day, W. F. (1971, September). Methodological problems in the analysis of behavior controlled by private events: Some unusual recommendations. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C. (Reprinted in Leigland, S. (Ed.) (1992). Radical behavorism: Willard Day on psychology and philosophy (pp. 13–58). Reno, NV: Context Press.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dougherty, M. A. (1980). Analyzing the concept of baseline and innovative methodology of the Redbook Study from a radical behaviorist perspective. Unpublished master’s thesis, California State University, Sacramento.

  • Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (1991). A standard methodology for the analysis, recording, and control of verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 49–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, R. (1985). The spontaneous interconnection of three repertoires. The Psychological Record, 35, 131–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B. (1979). A laboratory model of psychotherapy: The boundary between clinical practice and experimental psychology. In P. Sjoden, S. Bates, & W. Dockens (Eds.), Trends in behavior therapy (pp. 23–38). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Galbicka, G. (1994). Shaping in the 21st century: Moving percentile schedules into applied settings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 739–760.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greenspoon, J. (1955). The reinforcing effect of two spoken sounds on the frequency of two responses. American Journal of Psychology, 68, 409–416.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (1986). The case of the silent dog: Verbal reports and the analysis of rules. A review of Ericsson and Simon’s Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 351–363.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (Ed.) (1989). Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Zettle, R. D., & Rosenbarb, I. (1989). Rule following. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 191–220). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1966). Superstition: A corollary of the principles of operant conditioning. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 33–51). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holz, W. C., & Azrin, N. H. (1966). Conditioning human verbal behavior. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 790–826). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyten, C. & Chase, P. N. (1991). An analysis of self-editing: Method and preliminary findings. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior (pp. 67–81). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlenberg, R. J., & Tsai, M. (1991). Functional analytic psychotherapy: Creating intense and curative therapeutic relationships. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlenberg, R. J., & Tsai, M. (1994). Improving cognitive therapy for depression with functional analytic psychotherapy: Theory and case study. The Behavior Analyst, 17, 305–319.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leigland, S. (1989). On the relation between radical behaviorism and the science of verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 7, 25–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leigland, S. (Ed.) (1992). Radical behavorism: Willard Day on psychology and philosophy. Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leigland, S. (1993). The case against physicalism in the analysis of behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 351–355.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, G., & Pear, J. (1992). Behavior modification: What it is and how to do it (4th Ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1984). Verbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 363–376.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. (1991). A retrospective appreciation of Willard Day’s contributions to radical behaviorism and the analysis of verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 97–104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ono, K. (1987). Superstitious behavior in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 261–271.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Place, U. T. (1991). Conversation analysis and the analysis of verbal behavior. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior (pp. 85–109). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfarb, I. S. (1992). A behavior analytic interpretation of the therapeutic relationship. The Psychological Record, 42, 341–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salzinger, K. (1991). Cognitive problems, behavioral solutions. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior (pp. 183–196). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salzinger, K., Portnoy, S., Zlotogura, P., & Keisner, R. (1963). The effect of reinforcement on continuous speech and on plural nouns in grammatical context. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 477–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A., & Kaplan, C. A. (1989). Foundations of cognitive science. In M. Posner (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive science (pp. 1–47). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1931). The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 427–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1932). On the rate of formation of a conditioned reflex. Journal of General Psychology, 7, 274–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1935). The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. Journal of General Psychology, 12, 40–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review, 52, 270–277, 291–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). ‘Superstition’ in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: The Free Press/Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1956). A case history in scientific method. American Psychologist, 11, 221–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1976). Farewell, my LOVELY! Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, 218.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1986). Some thoughts about the future. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 299–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1989). Recent issues in the analysis of behavior. Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verplanck, W. S. (1955). The control of the content of conversation: Reinforcement of statements of opinion. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 668–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanck, W. S. (1992). Verbal concept “mediators” as simple operants. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 45–68.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, W. C., & Verplanck, W. S. (1956). Some observations on the reinforcement of verbal operants. American Journal of Psychology, 69, 448–451.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sam Leigland.

Additional information

Comments by Jay Moore and David Schmitt on an earlier draft of this paper are greatly appreciated. Special thanks are also due to Mark Sundberg, H. McIlvaine Parsons, an two anonymous reviewers for detailed and constructive comments.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Leigland, S. An experimental analysis of ongoing verbal behavior: Reinforcement, verbal operants, and superstitious behavior. Analysis Verbal Behav 13, 79–104 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392908

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392908

Navigation