Skip to main content
Log in

Toward a Science of History

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The scientific status of History was compared to other sciences in the critical areas event selection, investigative operations, and theory construction. First, in terms of events studied, history is regarded as a quasi-scientific study of past events. However, viewed from the science of behavior’s perspective of what historians actually do, history becomes a study of current records. As a study of currently existing records, not the non-existent past, history has potential to become a science. Second, like other scientists, historians may undertake manipulative investigations: they can locate the presence and absence of a condition in records and thereby determine its relation to other recorded phenomena. A limitation has been the lack of quantification that results from emphasis on the uniqueness of things rather than on their communality. Scientific training would facilitate viewing similar things as instances of a larger class that could be counted. Another limitation that cannot be easily overcome is the inability to produce raw data. This limitation has created problems in theoretical practices, the third area of comparison, because theoretical constructions have frequently been substituted for missing data. This problem too could be reduced through scientific training, particularly in other behavior sciences. An authentic science of history is possible.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Atkinson, R. F. Knowledge and explanations in history: An introduction to the philosophy of history. Ithaca: Cornell, 1978.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Beard, C. A. Written history as an act of faith. American Historical Review. 1939, 2, 219–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, R. C. Macaulay. Norman, Oklahoma: University of Oklahoma Press, 1938.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, J. B. The art of history. London: Methuen & Co. Ltd., 1926.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S. Motivation. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bury, J. B. The science of history. In Selected essays. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1930.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, G. Kitson. The critical historian. New York: Basic Books, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collingwood, R. G. The idea of history. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1946.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, A. H., & Meyer, J. R. The economics of slavery in the ante bellum south. Journal of Political Economy, 1958, 66, 95–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croce, Beneditto. History: Its theory and practice. NY: Russell & Russell, Inc., 1960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuneo, Ernest. Science and history. New York: Duell, Bloan and Pearce, 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Coulanges, Fustel. An inaugural lecture. In F. Stearn (Ed.), The varieties of history. New York: Meridian, 1957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dray, W. H. (Ed.). Philosophical analysis and history. New York: Harper & Row, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, R. W., & Engermann, S. L. Time on the cross: The economics of American negro slavery and evidence and methods—a supplement. Boston and Toronto: Little, Brown and Co., 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hearst, E. (Ed.). The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegal, G. W. H. [The philosophy of history] (J. Sibree, trans.). NY: Wiley, 1944.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 1970, 13, 243–266.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Boring, E. G. (Eds.), A source book in the history of psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holborn, Hajo. History and the humanities. NY: Doubleday & Company, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, H. S. History as art and as science. NY: Harper & Row Publishers, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, H. M. Animal learning and behavior theory. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. The nature of psychology as a natural science. Acta Psychologia, 1953.

  • Kantor, J. R. Psychology and logic (Vol. 1). Chicago: The Principia Press, 1945.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. The logic of modern science. Chicago: The Principia Press, 1953.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. The nature of psychology as a natural science. Acta Psychologica, 1938, 4, 1–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. The scientific evolution of psychology (Vol. 1). Chicago: The Principia Press, 1963.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. The scientific evolution of psychology (Vol. II). Chicago: The Principia Press, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. Interbehavioral philosophy. Chicago: The Principia Press, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maher, B. A., & Maher, W. B. Psychopathology. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maritain, Jacques. On the philosophy of history. Clifton, NJ: Augustus M. Kelley Publishers, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meiland, Jack W. Sceptism and historical knowledge. New York: Random House, 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  • Namier, L. B. History and political culture. In F. Stern (Ed.), The varieties of history. New York: Meridian, 1957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakeshott, Michael. Experience and its modes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1933.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rouse, A. L. The use of history. London: Hodder & Staughton Limited, 1946.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rouse, I. Introduction to prehistory: A systematic approach. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co., 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salmon, Lucy Maynard. Why is history rewritten? New York: Oxford University Press, 1929.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shafer, R. J. A guide to historical method. Homewood, IL.: Dorsey Press, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shotwell, James T. The faith of an historian and other essays. NY: Walker and Company, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, William. History as applied science. Detroit: Wayne State University Press, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevelyan, G. M. An autobiography and other essays. New York: Longmans, Green and Co., Ltd., 1949.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevor-Roper, H. Historical essays. London: Macmillan, 1957.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Parrott, L.J., Hake, D.F. Toward a Science of History. BEHAV ANALYST 6, 121–132 (1983). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392391

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392391

Navigation