Skip to main content
Log in

Toward a Functional Analysis of Delusional Speech and Hallucinatory Behavior

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

An approach to a functional analysis of delusional speech and hallucinatory behavior is described and discussed using concepts found in Goldiamond’s (1975a and 1984) nonlinear contingency analysis and Skinner’s Verbal Behavior (1957). This synthesis draws upon and concords with research from the animal laboratory, with the extensive experimental literatures on stimulus control and signal detection theory, and with our own clinical experiences.

In this formulation, delusional speech and hallucinatory behavior are viewed as successful operants. Accordingly, we argue that such behaviors can be considered adaptive and rational, rather than maladaptive and irrational, when analyzed within a model of consequential governance that includes alternative sets of contingencies. Several clinical examples are offered to illustrate both analytic procedures and the design of systemic treatment programs based upon a behavioral contingency analysis derived from a natural science of behavior. Throughout, we emphasize the consequential governance of these clinically important classes of behavior, in contrast to other approaches which suggest formal similarities to operant verbal behavior but largely ignore the role of consequential contingencies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adler, A. (1931). What life should mean to you. New York: Putnam & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aesop (620–560 BC). The little boy who cried wolf. In Aesop’s Fables: Illustrated Junior Library, (p. 132) Grosset & Dunlap, New York (1947).

    Google Scholar 

  • Andronis, P. T. (1983). Symbolic aggression by pigeons: Contingency coadduction. Ph.D. dissertation. The University of Chicago.

  • Andronis, P. T., Goldiamond, I., & Layng, T. V. J. (1983). “Symbolic aggression” by pigeons: Contingency coadduction. Paper presented at the Ninth Annual Convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee, WI, May 1983.

  • Andronis, P. T., & Goldiamond, I. (1983). Dominance, submission, and cooperation by pigeons. Paper presented at the Ninth Annual Convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee, WI, May 1983.

  • Braginsky, B. M., Braginsky, D. D., & Ring, K. (1969). Methods of madness: The mental hospital as last resort. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braginsky, B. M., Grosse, G., & Ring, K. (1966). Controlling outcomes through impression management: An experimental study of the manipulative tactics of mental patients. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 30, 295–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, C. E. S., Heiby, E. M., & Tharp, R. G. (1983). A verbal behavior analysis of auditory hallucinations. The Behavior Analyst, 6, 133–143.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cullen, C., Hattersley, J., & Tennant, L. (1981). Establishing behaviour: The constructional approach. In G. Davey (Ed.), Applications of conditioning theory (pp. 149–161). London: Methuen & Co., Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cullen, J. (Ed.) (1974). Experimental behavior: A basis for the study of mental disturbance. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunham, P. J., & Grantmyre, J. (1982). Changes in a multiple-response repertoire during response contingent punishment and response restriction: sequential relationships. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 123–134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, R. (1983). Resurgence of previously reinforced behavior during extinction. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 3, 391–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, R., & Skinner, B. F. (1980). Resurgence of responding after the cessation of response-independent reinforcement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 77, 6251–6253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fairweather, G. W., Sanders, D. H., Maynard, H., & Cressler, D. L. (1969). Community life for the mentally ill. Chicago: Aldine Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk, J. L. (1971). The nature and determinants of adjunctive behavior. Physiology and Behavior, 6, 577–588.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, J. L. (1977). The origins and functions of adjunctive behavior. Animal Learning and Behavior, 5, 325–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B. (1972). The experimental analysis of clinical phenomena. The Psychological Record, 22, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredriksen, L. W., & Peterson, G. L. (1977). Schedule induced aggression in humans and animals: a comparative parametric review. Aggressive Behavior, 3, 57–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, B. J. (1974). The subjective experience of perceptual and cognitive disturbances in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 30, 333–340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gambrill, E. (1977). Behavior modification: Handbook for application and assessment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garza, R. de la, & Johanson, C. E. (1983). The discriminative stimulus properties of cocaine in the rhesus monkey. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 19, 145–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1962). Perception. In A. Bachrach (Ed.), Experimental foundations of clinical psychology (pp. 280–340). New York: Basic Books, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1964). Response bias in perceptual communication. Disorders of communication, XLII, Research Publications, Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 334–363.

  • Goldiamond, I. (1970). Human control over human behavior. In M. Wertheimer (Ed.), Confrontation (pp. 254–306). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1974). Toward a constructional approach to social problems: Ethical and constitutional issues raised by applied behavior analysis. Behaviorism, 2, 1–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1975a). Alternative sets as a framework for behavioral formulations and research. Behaviorism, 3, 49–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1975b). Insider-outsider problems: A constructional approach. Rehabilitation Psychology, 22, 103–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1975c). A constructional approach to self-control. In A. Schwartz and I. Goldiamond (Eds.), Social casework: A behavioral approach (pp. 67–130). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1976). Protection of human subjects and patients: A social contingency analysis of distinctions between research and practice, and its implications. Behaviorism, 4, 1–41.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1979). Behavioral approaches and liaison psychiatry. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 2, 379–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1984). Training parent trainers and ethecists in nonlinear analysis of behavior. In Dangle, R. F. & Polster, A. (Eds.), Foundations of research and practice, (pp. 504–545) New York: Gilford press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I., & Dyrud, J. E. (1967). Some applications and implication of behavioral analysis for psychotherapy. In J. Schlien (Ed.), Research in psychotherapy (pp. 54–89), Vol. III. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I., & Hawkins, W. A. (1958). Vexierversuch: the log relationship between word-frequency and recognition obtained in the absence of stimulus words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 457–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. and Thompson, D. (1968). The functional analysis of behavior. Institute for Behavioral Research, Silver Springs, Maryland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, L. (1950). Verbal hallucinations as automatic speech: The reactivation of dormant speech habit. American Journal of Psychiatry, 107, 110–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1973). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Second Edition. Huntington, NY: R. E. Krieger Pub. Co., Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, S. (1983). Animal models in schizophrenia research. In G. Davey (Ed.), Animal models of human behavior (pp. 315–338). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isaacs, W., Thomas, J., & Goldiamond, I. (1960). Application of operant conditioning to reinstate verbal behavior in psychotics. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 25, 8–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keehn, J. D. (Ed.) (1979). Origins of madness: Psychopathology in animal life. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimmel, K. D. (Ed.) (1971). Experimental psychopathology: Recent research and theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Layng, T. V. J., Merley, S., Cohen, J., Andronis, P. T., and Layng, M. (1976). Programed instruction, self-control, and in-patient psychiatry. Educational Resource Clearing House (ERIC), Document listing #142 886.

  • Layng, T. V. J., Goldiamond, I., & Andronis, P. T. (1983). Social behaviors and behaviors of social consequence: Headbanding by pigeons. Paper presented at the Ninth Annual Convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee, WI, May 1983.

  • Looney T. A., & Cohen P. S. (1982). Aggression induced by intermittent positive reinforcement. Biobehavioral Reviews, 6, 15–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, D. O. (1982). Concurrent behaviors: are the interpretations mutually exclusive? The Behavior Analyst, 5, 175–187.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mauldin, M., Karp, H., & Layng, T. V. J. (1979). The discrimination of sexy and friendly interpersonal cues: A signal detection analysis. Paper presented at the Fifth Annual Convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Dearborn, MI, June 1979.

  • McGuigan, F. (1966). Covert oral behavior and auditory hallucinations. Psychophysiology, 3, 73–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merley, S., & Layng, T. V. J. (1976). In-patient psychiatry and programed instruction: Application and research in constructional theory. Improving Human Performance Quarterly, 5, 35–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. L. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149–155.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, G., & Lentz, R. (1977). Psychosocial treatment of chronic mental patients: Milieu versus social-learning programs. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1903). Psychopathology and psychiatry: Selected works. Moscow: Foreign Language Publishing House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, G. S. (1961). Attention in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 57–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Salzinger, K. (1973). Schizophrenia: Behavioral aspects. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuster, C. R., Fischman, M. W., & Johanson, C. E. (1981). Internal stimulus control and subjective effects of drugs. Behavioral Pharmacology of Human Drug Dependence, 37, Research Monograph Series, National Institute on Drug Abuse, 116–129.

  • Sidman, M. (1961). Normal sources of pathological behavior. Science, 132, 61–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1936). The verbal summator and a method for the study of latent speech. Journal of Psychology, 2, 71–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R. (1977). Schedule-induced behavior. In Honig, W. and Staddon, J. E. R. (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 125–152). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoddard, L. T., & Sidman, M. (1971). The removal and restoration of stimulus control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 16, 143–154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tennant, L., Cullen, C., & Hattersley, J. (1981). Applied behaviour analysis: Intervention with retarded people. In G. Davey (Ed.), Applications of conditioning theory (pp. 29–58). London: Methuen & Co., Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1924). Behaviorism. New York: The People’s Institute Publishing Company, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zubin, J., & Hunt, H. F. (Eds.) (1967). Comparative psychopathology: Animal and human. New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

We fondly dedicate this paper to Izzy Goldiamond, whose friendship and writings have pervaded our work almost from the outset.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Layng, T.V.J., Andronis, P.T. Toward a Functional Analysis of Delusional Speech and Hallucinatory Behavior. BEHAV ANALYST 7, 139–156 (1984). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391897

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391897

Navigation