Skip to main content
Log in

Mother’s, father’s regulation and child’s self-regulation in a computer-mediated learning situation

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study has explored the parents’ regulation strategies that were more likely to support children’s self-regulation in learning situations with computers. These strategies have been analysed by means of new grids involving seven categories of behaviour: cognitive strategies relating to identification of objective, exploration of means, attention and evaluation; motivational strategies; and socio-communicative strategies such as joint attention and request. 62 7-year-old children from Quebec families were examined with their mothers and fathers. The children were asked to complete two learning tasks that involved using LOGO software to draw a picture. Results indicated that parents’ regulation varied depending on their gender; a higher directivity was observed in mothers, more specifically through some specific strategies. Analysis of variance on repeated measures showed that, during the session, there was a decrease in the specification of the objective, the initiation of joint attention and of behaviour regulation in parents; and an improvement of the identification of the objective, planning and self-evaluation in children. Correlations between parental regulation and children’s self-regulation strategies indicated positive links concerning strategies relating to joint attention and to motivation; and negative links concerning strategies relating to the exploration of means and evaluation. A good adjustment of the parents’ regulation and the children’s self-regulation correlated positivel with success in the task. Methodological implications are proposed for research and psycho-educative intervention.

Résumé

Cette étude a investigué quelles stratégies parentales sont plus susceptibles de soutenir l’autorégulation d’enfants en situation d’apprentissage médiatisé par l’ordinateur. Ces stratégies sont analysées au moyen de grilles intégrant: des stratégies cognitives d’identification de l’objectif, de planification et d’exploration de moyens, d’attention et d’évaluation; des stratégies motivationnelles; des stratégies socio-communicatives d’attention conjointe et de requête. 62 enfants québécois de 7 ans ont été examinés avec leurs mères et pères. L’apprentissage consistait à réaliser 2 dessins au moyen du logiciel LOGO. Les résultats ont indiqué que: la régulation des parents variait en fonction de leur sexe dans le sens d’une plus grande directivité maternelle mais particulièrement pour certaines stratégies. L’analyse de variance sur mesures répétées a montré qu’au cours de la séance, les parents diminuaient le rappel de l’objectif, l’initiation d’attention conjointe et de régulation du comportement; et les enfants amélioraient leur identification de l’objectif, leur planification et leur évaluation. Des relations positives sont obtenues entre les stratégies d’attention conjointe et de motivation respectives des enfants et des parents, des relations négatives sont obtenues quant aux stratégies respectives d’exploration et d’évaluation. Un bon ajustement de la régulation parentale et l’autorégulation des enfants sont liés positivement à la réussite à la tâche. Des implications méthodologiques sont dégagées pour la recherche et l’intervention psycho-éducative.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Agran, M. (1997).Student-directed learning: Teaching self-determination skills. Pacific Grove: CA: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A.B. (1997).Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker-Sennett, J., Matusov, E., & Rogoff, B. (1993). Planning as a developmental process. In H.W. Reese (Eds.),Advances in child development and behaviour (vol. 24, pp. 253–281). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, P.J., Schwade, J.A., Wewerka, S.S., & Delaney, K. (1999). Planning ahead: goal-directed problem-solving by 2-year-olds.Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1321–1337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, D.C., & Bell, L.G. (1989). Micro and macro measurement of family systems concepts.Journal of Family Psychology, 3(2), 137–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boekaerts, M. (1996). Self-regulated learning at the junction of cognition and motivation.European Psychologist, 1(2), 100–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boekaerts, M. (2002). Bringing about change in the classroom: Strengths and weakness of self-regulated learning approach.Learning and Instruction, 12, 589–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronson, M.B. (2001). Self-regulation in early childhood: Nature and nurture. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, other more mysterious mechanisms. In F.E. Weinert & R.H. Kluwe (Eds.),Metacognition, motivation and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruder, S., Perels, F., & Schmitz, B. (2004). Self-regulation and parental homework support. The evaluation of a parent training for secondary school children.Zeitschrift fur Entwicklungspsychologie und Padagogische Psychologie, 36(3), 139–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J.S. (1983).Le développement de l’enfant; Savoir faire, savoir dire. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bynum, M.S., & Brody, G.H. (2005). Coping behaviors, parenting, and perceptions of children’s internalising and externalising problems in rural African American mothers.Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 54(1), 58–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conner, D.B., & Cross, D.R. (2003). Longitudinal analysis of the presence, efficacy and stability of maternal scaffolding during informal problem-solving interactions.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(3), 315–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conner, D.B., Knight, D.K., & Cross, D.R. (1997). Mothers’ an fathers’ scaffolding of their 2-year-olds during problem-solving and literacy interactions.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(3), 323–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCooke, P.A., & Brownell, C.A. (1999). Young children’s help-seeking in mastery-oriented contexts.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41(2), 229–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • De la Ossa, J.L., & Gauvain, M. (2001). Joint attention by mothers and children while using plans.International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 176–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbers, E., Maier, R., Hoekstra, T., & Hoogsteder, M. (1992). Internalization and adult-child interaction.Learning and Instruction, 2, 101–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eimerl, K. (1993).L’informatique éducative. Cheminement dans l’apprentissage. Paris: Armand Colin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, B.I., & Gauvain, M. (1997). Mother-child problem solving: Continuity through the early childhood years.Developmental psychology, 33(3), 480–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H. (1987). Speculation about the nature and development of metacognition. In F. Weinert & R. Kluwe (Eds.),Metacognition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 21–29). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Focant, J., Grégoire, J., & Desoete, A. (in press). Goal-setting, planning and control strategies and arithmetical problem solving at grade 5. In A. Desoete & M. Veenman (Eds.),Metacognition in mathematics education. Nova Science: Hauppage, NY.

  • Freund, L.S. (1990). Maternal regulation of children’s problem-solving and its impact on children’s performance.Child Development, 61, 113–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, S.L., & Scholnick, E.K. (1997).The developmental psychology of planning: Why, how, and when do we plan? Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, W., & Rogoff, B. (1990). Children’s deliberateness of planning according to task circumstances.Developmental Psychology, 26, 480–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M. (1995). Influence of purpose of an interaction on adult-child planning.Infancia y Aprendizaje, 70, 141–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M. (1999). Everyday opportunities for the development of planning skills: sociocultural and family influences. In A. Goncu (Ed.),Children’s engagement in the world: sociocultural perspectives (pp. 173–201). New-York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M., & Rogoff, B. (1989). Collaborative problem solving and children’s planning skills.Developmental Psychology, 25, 139–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M., de la Ossa, J.L., & Hurtado-Ortiz, M.T. (2001). Parental guidance as children learn to use cultural tools: The case of pictorial plans.Cognitive Development, 16(1), 551–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilmore, L., Cuskelly, M., & Hayes, A. (2003). Self-regulatory behaviors in children with Down syndrome and typically developing children measured using the Goodman Lock Box.Research in Developmental Disabilities, 24, 95–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1987). Balancing for weakness and challenging developmental potential: A longitudinal study of mother-infants dyads in apprenticeships interactions.Developmental Psychology, 23(6), 762–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1988). Becoming aware of one’s competence in the second year: Developmental progression within the mother-child dyad.International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11(3), 305–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, B.B. (1984a). Parents and exploration: The effects of the context on individual differences in exploratory behavior.Child Development, 55, 1237–1245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, B.B. (1984b). Social support and exploration.Child Development, 55, 1246–1251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J.A., & Fivush, R. (1991). Planning in the preschool years: The emergence of plans from general event knowledge.Cognitive Development, 6, 393–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J.A., Shapiro, L.R., & Sosa, B.B. (1995). Planning in the real world: Preschool children’s scripts and plans for familiar events.Child Development, 66, 984–998.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J.A., Sosa, B.B., & Shapiro, L.R. (1997).Scripts and plans: The development of preschool children’s event knowledge and event planning. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, A., & Issroff, K. (2004). Learning technologies: Affective and social issues in computer-supported collaborative learning.Computers and Education, 44(4), 395–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labrell, F. (1996). Interactions de tutelle paternelle et maternelle avec le jeune enfant: La sollicitation de l’autonomie dans la deuxième année.Enfance, 4, 447–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lebrun, M. (1999).Des technologies pour enseigner et apprendre. Bruxelles: De Boeck.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2002). The development of self-regulated learning in elementary school children: Association between task difficulty and allocation of study time.Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 49(1), 3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithaug D.K., & Mithaug D.E. (2003). The effects of choice opportunities and self-regulation training on the self-engagement and learning of young children with disabilities (pp. 141–157). In Mithaug, Mithaug, Agran, Martin, & Wehmeyer (2003).Self-determined learning theory: Construction, verification and evaluation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss, E. (1986). Interactions mères-enfants et différences intellectuelles individuelles.Apprentissage et Socialisation, 9, 143–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss, E. (1990). Social interaction and metacognitive development in gifted preschoolers.Gifted Child Quarterly, 34, 16–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullis, R.L., & Mullis, A.K. (1986). Mother-child and Father-child interactions: A study of problem-solving strategies.Child Study Journal, 16, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nader-Grosbois, N. (2004). Elaboration of a self-regulation and hetero-regulation coding grid: Interest for the approach of persons with atypical development. InActs of the 5th European Conference Psychological Theory and Research on mental retardation and cognitive developmental disabilities (pp. 74–82) (Lille, may 27–29, 2004).

  • Nader-Grosbois, N. (2006a). Grille d’analyse des stratégies autorégulatrices et hétérorégulatrices: Intérêt pour l’intervention auprès des personnes à développement atypique. In Gascon, Poulin, Detraux, Boisvert, & Haelewyck (Eds.),Déficience intellectuelle: Savoirs et perspectives d’action. Tome 2: Formation, interventions, adaptation et soutien social (pp. 263–278). Québec: Presses Inter Universitaires.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nader-Grosbois, N. (2006b). Comment identifier l’autorégulation chez des enfants à développement atypique? In Schneider, Mietkiewicz, Bouyer, Dollander, Rodrigues-Martins, & Toniolo (Eds.),Enfant en développement, famille et handicaps. Interactions et transmissions (chap. 22). Toulouse: Erès.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nader-Grosbois, N. (2007). Régulation,autorégulation, dysrégulation: Pistes pour l’intervention et la recherche. Wavre: Mardaga.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nader-Grosbois, N. (to be published). Early self-regulation in children with intellectual disabilities during a situation of assessment.Arobase.

  • Nader-Grosbois, N., & Thomée, C. (2006). Comment étudier l’autorégulation d’enfants présentant un retard mental et l’hétérorégulation parentale en situation d’apprentissage médiatisée et non médiatisée? In Schneider, Mietkiewicz, Bouyer, Dollander, Rodrigues-Martins, & Toniolo (Eds.),Enfant en développement, famille et handicaps, Interactions et transmissions (chap. 25). Toulouse: Erès.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson-Le Gall, S., Kratzer, L., Jones, E., & DeCooke, P. (1990). Children’s self-assessment of performance and task-related help seeking.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 245–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Normandeau, S. (1992). Développement des stratégies cognitives et des habiletés d’autocontrôle chez les enfants de 8 à 14 ans.Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 46, 117–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Normandeau, S., & Arsenault, J. (1994). Effect of mother’s familiarity with a formal task on cognitive monitoring during mother-child interactions.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58(1), 46–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Normandeau, S., Larivée, S., Tremblay, R.E., Gagnon, C., Charlebois, P., & Bouffard-Bouchard, T. (1992). Cognitive monitoring in a joint activity between children and their non expert parents on a formal operation task.Learning and Individual Differences, 4, 73–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • North, A.S. & Noyes, J.M. (2002). Gender influences on children’s computer attitudes and cognition.Computers in Human Behavior, 18(2), 135–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papert, S. (1981).Jaillissement de l’esprit. Ordinateurs et apprentissage. Tours: Flammarion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parrila, R.K., Aeystoe, S., & Das, J.P. (1994). Development of planning in relation to age, attention, simultaneous and successive processing.Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 12(3), 212–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parrila, R.K., Das, J.P., & Dash, U.N. (1996). Development of planning and its relation to other cognitive processes.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17, 597–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinard, A. (1986). “Prise de Conscience” and taking charge of one’ own cognitive functioning.Human Development, 29(6), 341–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P.R. (1999). The role of motivation in promoting and sustaining self-regulated learning.International Journal of Educational Research, 31, 459–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, M.W., Kerig, P., & Cowan, P.A. (1988). Mothers and fathers teaching 3 years old: An authoritative parenting and adult scaffolding of young children’s learning.Developmental Psychology, 24, 832–839.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, M., Green, D., McVicar, J., & Bountrogianni, M. (1992). The mathematical parent: Parental scaffolding, parenting style, and learning outcomes in long-division mathematics homework.Journal of applied Developmental Psychology, 13, 17–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M. (1995). More about the development of self-regulation: Complex, long-term and thoroughly social.Educational Psychologist, 30(4), 207–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prevost, R.A., Bronson, M.B., & Casey, M.B. (1995). Planning processes in preschool children.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 505–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puustinen, M. (1998). Help-seeking behavior in a problem-solving situation: Development of self-regulation.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 13(2), 271–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Retschitzchi, J., & Gurtner, J.L. (1996).L’enfant et l’ordinateur. Aspects psychologiques et pédagogiques des nouvelles technologies de l’information. Liège: Mardaga.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990).Apprenticeship in thinking; Cognitive development in social context. New-York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., & Wertsch, J. (1984a).Children’s learning in the zone of proximal development. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Ellis, S., & Gardner, W. (1984b). Adjustment of adult-child instruction according to child’s age and task.Developmental Psychology, 20, 193–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, G., & Russell, A. (1987). Mother-child and father-child relationship in middle childhood.Child Development, 58, 1573–1585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G.B., Geathart, M., & Guberman, S.R. (1984). The social organization of early number development.News Directions for Child Development, 23, 19–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D.H., & Zimmerman, B.J. (1994).Self-regulation of learning and performance. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwier, R.A. (1993). Learning environments and interaction for emerging technologies: Implications for learner control and practice.Canadian Journal of educational Communication, 22(3), 163–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, I.W., & Ippolito, M.F. (1997). Goal-directedness and its relation to inhibitory control among toddlers.Infant Behaviour and Development, 20(2), 271–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, S.P., & Kumari, P. (2001). Parental inducement of self-regulation, strategy use and attribution of success/failure among school children.Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied psychology, 27(1–2), 169–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,57(1).

  • St-Laurent, D., & Moss, E. (2002). Le développement de la planification: Influence d’une activité conjointe.Enfance, 4, 341–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szepkouski, G.M., Gauvain, M., & Carberry, M. (1994). The development of planning skills in children with and without mental retardation.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15(2), 187–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomée, C., & Nader-Grosbois, N. (2004). Self-regulation in children with intellectual disabilities in computer-mediated/material-mediated learning situations and parents’ hetero-regulation. InActs of the 5th European Conference Psychological theory and Research on mental retardation and cognitive developmental disabilities (pp. 88–93), (Lille, may 27–29, 2004).

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1978).Mind in Society. The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, M.C., & Pennington, B.F. (1988). Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children: Views from developmental psychology.Developmental Neuropsychology, 4, 199–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, M.C. (1991). Rule-guided behavior and self-monitoring on the Tower of Hanoï disk-transfer task.Cognitive Development, 6(1), 59–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J.V., Mc Namee, G.D., Mc Lane, J.B., & Budwig, N.A. (1980). The adult-child dyad as a problem-solving system.Child Development, 51, 1215–1221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J.V. (1985).Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westerman, M.A. (1990). Coordination of maternal directives with preschoolers, behaviors in compliance problem and healthy dyad.Developmental Psychology, 26, 621–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P.H., & Stockley, D.B. (1998). Computing Technologies as sites for developing self-regulated learning. In D.H. Schunk & B.J. Zimmerman, (Eds.),Self-regulated learning: From teaching to self reflective practices. New-York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winnykamen, F. (1993). Gestion socio-cognitive du recours à l’aide d’autrui chez l’enfant.Journal International de Psychologie, 28, 645–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolters, C.A. (1999). The relation between high school students’ motivational regulation and their use of learning strategies, effort and classroom performance.Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 281–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolters, C.A. (2003). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating an underemphasized aspect of self-regulated learning.Educational Psychologist, 38(4), 189–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D., & Wood, H. (1996). Commentary: Contingency in tutoring and learning.Learning and Instruction, 6(4), 391–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, H., & Wood, D. (1999). Help seeking, learning and contingent tutoring.Computers and education, 33, 153–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D., & Middleton, D. (1975). A study of assisted problem solving.British Journal of Psychology, 66, 181–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D., Bruner, J.S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D., Wood, H., & Middleton, D. (1978). An experimental evaluation of four-faces to face teaching strategies.International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2, 131–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B.J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: a social cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P.R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.),Handbook of self-regulation: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 13–41). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nader-Grosbois, N., Normandeau, S., Ricard-Cossette, M. et al. Mother’s, father’s regulation and child’s self-regulation in a computer-mediated learning situation. Eur J Psychol Educ 23, 95–115 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173142

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173142

Key words

Navigation