Skip to main content
Log in

Interactions between children’s metacognitive abilities, working memory capacity, strategies and performance during problem-solving

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper reports two related studies intended to explore the interactions between children’s metacognitive abilities, their working memory capacity, the development and selection of strategies and their performance on problem-solving tasks. In the first study, a sample of 20 children aged 5 and 6 were presented with a reclassification task. In the second study, a sample of 72 children aged 6, 8 and 10 were presented with a multidimensional discrimination learning (MDL) task. Data was collected related to the children’s metacognitive abilities, working memory capacity, response strategies and task performance. The results indicated that performance on both tasks was dependent upon developmentally changing interactions between these various aspects of cognitive functioning. In particular, the relationship of working memory capacity to performance was dependent upon metacognitive abilities. The results also suggested that metacognitive awareness did not directly affect performance, but that such a relationship was dependent upon the development of strategic control. The implications of these results for understanding U-shaped behavioural growth and other common developmental patterns are discussed. Within the educational sphere, the study emphasises the significance and possibility for children as learners of fostering certain kinds of metacognitive ability.

Résumé

Cet article rapporte les résultats de deux recherches destinées à explorer les interactions entre capacités métacognitives de l’enfant, capacité de la mémoire de travail, développement et sélection de stratégies, et performances à des tâches de résolution de problème. Dans la première étude, un échantillon de 20 enfants âgés de 5 ou 6 ans, était soumis à une tâche de reclassification. Dans la deuxième recherche, un échantillon de 72 enfants âgés de 6, 8 ou 10 ans était confronté à une tâche d’apprentissage de discrimination multidimensionnelle. Les résultats montrent que les performances aux deux tâches, dépendent des changements développementaux dans l’interaction entre les différents aspects du fonctionnement cognitif cités plus haut et mesurés dans cette recherche. En particulier, les relations entre mémoire de travail et performance dépendent des compétences métacognitives. Les résultats montrent aussi que la conscience métacognitive n’affecte pas directement les performances, mais que la relation entre les deux dépend du développement du contrôle stratégiques. Les explications des ces résultats pour l’interprétation des patrons de développement courants ou des évolutions en forme de U sont discutées. Dans le champ de l’éducation, l’étude contribue à mettre en valeur l’intérêt et la possibilité d’encourager le développement de certains types de capacités métacognitives.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barringer, C., & Gholson, B. (1980). Experiment 8: Selective attention and information processing in normal and underachieving readers. In B. Gholson (Ed.)The cognitive-developmental basis of human learning: Studies in hypothesis testing (pp. 197–214). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berch, D.B., & Evans, R.C. (1973). Decision processes in children’s recognition memory.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16, 148–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D.F. (Ed.). (1990).Children’s strategies: Contemporary views of cognitive development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C.J. (1983). Working-memory systems and cognitive development. In C.J. Brainerd (Ed.),Recent advances in cognitive developmental theory (pp. 167–235). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L. (1978). Knowing when, where and how to remember: A problem of metacognition. In R. Glaser (Ed.),Advances in instructional psychology (vol. 1, pp. 77–165). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation and other more mysterious mechanisms. In F.E. Weinert & R.H. Kluwe (Eds.),Metacognition, motivation & understanding (pp. 65–116). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L., & DeLoache, J.S. (1978). Skills, plans and self-regulation. In R.S. Siegler (Ed.),Children’s thinking: What develops? (pp. 3–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J.S., Goodnow, J.J., & Austin, G.A. (1956).A Study of thinking. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P.E., & Trabasso, T. (1971). Transitive inference and memory in young children.Nature, 232, 456–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campione, J.C. (1987). Metacognitive components of instructional research with problem learners. In F.E. Weinert & R.H. Kluwe (Eds.),Metacognition, motivation & understanding (pp. 117–140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1985).Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • English, L. (1992). Children’s use of domain-specific knowledge and domain-general strategies in novel problem solving.British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 203–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H. (1978). Metacognitive development. In J.M. Scandura & C.J. Brainerd (Eds.),Structural/process models of complex human behaviour (pp. 213–245). Alphen a.d. Rijn, The Netherlands: Sijthoff & Noordhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive developmental inquiry.American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H., Beach, D.R., & Chinsky, J.M. (1966). Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age.Child Development, 37, 283–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gholson, B. (Ed.). (1980).The cognitive-developmental basis of human learning: Studies in hypothesis testing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gholson, B., Levine, M., & Phillips, S. (1972). Hypotheses, strategies and stereotypes in discrimination learning.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 13, 423–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, R., & Pellegrino, J.W. (1987). Aptitudes for learning and cognitive processes. In F.E. Weinert & R.H. Kluwe (Eds.),Metacognition, motivation & understanding (pp. 267–288). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G.S. (1980). A learning set approach to multiple classification: Evidence from a theory of cognitive levels.International Journal of Behavioural Development, 3, 409–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K.J., & Nisbett, R.E. (1988). Induction. In R.J. Sternberg & E.E. Smith (Eds.),The psychology of human thought (pp. 50–91). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1964).The early growth of logic in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemler, D.G. (1978). Patterns of hypothesis testing in children’s discriminative learning: A study of the development of problem-solving strategies.Developmental Psychology, 14, 653–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kyllonen, P.C., & Christal, R.E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?!Intelligence, 14, 389–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M. (1966). Hypothesis behaviour by humans during discrimination learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 331–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lunzer, E.A. (1968).The regulation of behaviour. London: Staples.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbet, J., & Shucksmith, J. (1986).Learning strategies. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget’s developmental stages.Acta Psychologica 32, 301–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, S., & Gholson, B. (1980). Experiment 7: Effects of explicit memory aids and coding demands upon problem solving. In B. Gholson (Ed.),The cognitive-developmental basis of human learning: Studies in hypothesis testing (pp. 183–196). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, S., & Levine, M. (1975). Probing for hypotheses with adults and children: Blank trials and introtacts.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 327–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Borkowski, J.G., & O’Sullivan, J. (1985). Children’s metamemory and the teaching of memory strategies. In D.L. Forrest-Pressley, G.E. MacKinnon, & T.G. Waller (Eds.),Metacognition, cognition, and human performance: Vol. 1. Theoretical perspectives (pp. 111–153). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, M.J., & Erdos, G. (1993). Strategy selection and metacognition.Educational Psychology, 13, 259–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scardamalia, M. (1977). Information processing capacity and the problem of horizontal “décalage”: A demonstration using combinatorial reasoning tasks.Child Development, 48, 28–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Weinert, F.E. (1989). Universal trends and individual differences in memory development. In A. De Ribaupierre (Ed.), Transition mechanisms in child development: The longitudinal perspective (pp. 68–106). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Weinert, F.E. (Eds.). (1990).Interactions among aptitudes, strategies and knowledge in cognitive performance. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M. (1978). The relationship between cognitive processes and the development of communication skills. In B. Kearey (Ed.),Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 1–42). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R.S., & Jenkins, E. (1989).How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: e Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. (1997). Concepts and methods for studying cognitive change. In E. Amsel & K.A. Renninger (Eds.),Change and development: Issues of theory, method and application (pp. 77–97). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiker, C.C., & Cantor, J.H. (1977). Introtacts as predictors of discrimination performance in kindergarten childrenJournal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 520–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, S., & Stavy, R. (Eds.) (1982).U-shaped behavioural growth. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tumblin, A., & Gholson, B. (1980). Experiment 5: Training attentional control: Effects of rule provision and instructional feedback upon the voluntary control of attention among Elementary school children. In B. Gholson (Ed.),The cognitive-developmental basis of human learning: Studies in hypothesis testing (pp. 159–174). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitebread, D. (1996). The development of childen’s strategies on an inductive reasoning task.British Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Youngman, M.B. (1976).Programmed methods for multivariate data (Version 5). Nottingham, U.K.: Nottingham University: School of Education.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Whitebread.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Whitebread, D. Interactions between children’s metacognitive abilities, working memory capacity, strategies and performance during problem-solving. Eur J Psychol Educ 14, 489–507 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172975

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172975

Key words

Navigation