Skip to main content
Log in

Prediction and control as determinants of behavioural uncertainty: Effects on task performance and heart rate reactivity

  • Papers
  • Published:
Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Control or control-belief is often viewed as being directly instrumental in facilitating coping mechanisms in aversive situations, and yet the empirical evidence for the beneficial effects of control is inconclusive. In this study we investigated the role of predictability in determining the effects of perceived control during an aversive reaction time task. Fifty-six subjects were allocated to one of four groups; predictable-control, predictable-no control, unpredictable-control, unpredictable-no control. In the predictable conditions, subjects could temporally predict the occurrence of an aversive noise. In the perceived control conditions, duration of the aversive tone was contingent on subject’s performance. All subjects were matched in terms of the nature of the task and in the number and time of receipt of both the warning signal and noise. Heart rate reactivity and two performance parameters were measured, reaction time and performance increase. Both predictability and control-belief led to a reduction in heart rate reactivity, although they appeared to function independently and at different points in the sequence of events. That is, predictability or perceived control was sufficient to mitigate the effects, of an aversive situation. Neither perception of control or predictability led to better task performance. These results are discussed in terms of behavioural uncertainty explanations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baker, S. R. (1994). The effects of control, feedback and predictability on psychophysiological indices of stress. Unpublished doctoral dissertation: University of Plymouth.

  • Bongard, S. (1995). Mental effort during active and passive coping: A dual-task analysis.Psychophysiology, 32: 242–248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bongard, S., and Hodapp, V. (1997). Active coping, work-pace, and cardiovascular responses: Evidence from laboratory studies.Journal of Psychophysiology, 11: 227–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bongard, S., Hodapp, V., Frisch, M., and Lennartz, K. (1994). Effects of active and passive coping of task performance and cardiovascular reactivity.Journal of Psychophysiology, 8: 219–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breier, A., Albus, M., Pickar, D., Zahn, T. P. Wolkowitz, O. M., and Paul, S. M. (1987). Controllable and uncontrollable stress in humans: Alterations in mood and neuroendocrine and psychophysiological function.American Journal of Psychiatry, 144: 1419–1425.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, J. M., and Arkin, R. M. (1980). Prediction, control, and learned helplessness.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38: 482–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, A. J., and Wilde, G. J. S. (1988). Effects of actual and potential stressor control on physiological and self-reported stress responses.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6: 371–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Contrada, R. J., Glass, D. C., Krakoff, L. R., Krantz, D. C., Kehoe, K., Collins, C., and Elting, E., (1982). Effects of control over aversive stimulation and Type A behaviour on cardiovascular and plasma catecholamine responses.Psychophysiology, 19: 408–419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corah, N. L., and Boffa, J. (1970). Perceived control, self-observation and response to aversive stimulation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16: 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeGood, D. E. (1975). Cognitive control factors in vascular stress responses.Psychophysiology, 12: 211–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, S. (1984).Stress and the perception of control. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geer, J. H., and Maisel, E. (1972). Evaluating the effects of the prediction-control confound.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23: 314–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glass, D. C., and Singer, J. E. (1972).Urban Stress: Experiments on Noise and Social Stressors. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodapp, V., Bongard, S., Heiligtag, U. (1992). Active coping, expression of anger, and cardiovascular reactivity.Personality and Individual Differences, 13: 1069–1076.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodapp, V., Heiligtag, U. and Stormer, S. W. (1990). Cardiovascular reactivity, anxiety, and anger during perceived controllability.Biological Psychology, 30: 161–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Light, K. C. (1981). Cardiovascular responses to effortful active coping: Implications for the role of stress in hypertension development.Psychophysiology, 18: 216–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Light, K. C., and Obrist, P. A. (1980). Cardiovascular response to stress: Effects of opportunity to avoid, shock experience and performance feedback.Psychophysiology, 17: 243–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovallo, W. R., Wilson, M. F., Pincomb, G. A., Edwards, G. L., Tompkins, P., and Brackett, D. J. (1985). Activation patterns to aversive stimulation in man: Passive exposure versus effort to control.Psychophysiology, 22: 283–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. T. (1959). Preliminary observations concerning the ‘Preception’, phenomenon.Psychophysiology Measurement Newsletter, 5: 2–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. T., Macindoe, I., and Tellegen, A. (1972). Preception: Autonomic response to shock as a function of predictability in time and locus.Psychophysiology, 9: 318–323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manuck, S. B., Harvey, A. H., Lechleiter, S. L., and Neal, K. S. (1978). Effects of coping on blood pressure responses to threat of aversive stimulation.Psychophysiology, 15: 544–549.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. M. (1980). When is a little information a dangerous thing? Coping with stressful events by monitoring versus blunting. In S. Levine, and H. Ursin (Eds.),Coping and Health, Proceedings of a NATO conference (pp. 145–169). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. M. (1979a). Controllability and human stress: Method, evidence and theory.Behaviour Research Therapy, 17: 287–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. M. (1979b). Coping with impending stress: Physiological and cognitive correlates of choice.Psychophysiology, 16: 572–581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., and Hendersen, R. W. (1985). Controllability and predictability, in acquired motivation.Annual Review of Psychology, 36: 495–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myrtek, M., and Foerster, F. (1986). The law of initial value: a rare exception.Biological Psychology, 22: 227–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nickels, J. B., Cramer, K. M., and Gural, D. M. (1992). Toward unconfounding prediction and control: Predictionless control made possible.Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 24: 156–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obrist, P. A., Gaeberlein, C. J., Teller, E. S., Langer, A. W., Grignolo, A., Light, K. C., and McCubbin, J. A. (1978). The relationship among heart rate, carotid dP/dt, and blood pressure in humans as a function of the type of stress.Psychophysiology, 15: 102–115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, C. C. (1968). An analysis of the concept of reinforcement.Psychological Review, 75: 155–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pervin, L. A. (1963). The need to predict and control under conditions of threat.Journal of Personality, 31: 570–587.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, M. L., Godaert, G. L. R., Ballieux, R. E., Vliet, M. V., Willemsen, J. J., Sweep, F. C. G. J. and Heijnen, C. J. (1998). Cardiovascular and endocrine responses to experimental stress: Effects of mental effort and controllability.Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23: 1–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, K. C. (1989). Psychophysiological consequences of behavioural choice in aversive situations. In A. Steptoe and A. Appels (Eds.),Stress, Personal Control and Health (pp. 239–256). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, K. C., Evans, P. D., Moran, P., and Price, J. (1986). Psychophysiological consequences of vigilant coping style.Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 39: A84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P., Maier, S. F. and Soloman, R. L. (1971). Unpredictable and uncontrollable aversive events. In F. R. Brush (Ed.),Aversive Conditioning and Learning (pp. 347–400). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siddle, D. A. T., and Turpin, G. (1980). Measurement, quantification and analysis of cardiac activity. In I. Martin, and P. H. Venables (Eds.),Techniques in Psychophysiology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steptoe, A. and Appels, A. (1989) (Eds).Stress, Personal Control and Health. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svebak, S. (1991). The role of effort in stress and emotion. In C. D. Spielberger and I. G. Sarason (Eds.),Stress and Emotion, Vol. 14 (pp. 121–134). New York: Hemisphere Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, R. M., and Leitten, C. L. (1993). Subjective, physiological, and behavioural effects of threat and challenge appraisal.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65: 248–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, J. M. (1971). Effects of coping behaviour in different warning signal conditions on stress pathology in rats.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 77: 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winefield, A. H. and Tiggemann, M. (1978). The effects of uncontrollable and unpredictable events on anagram solving.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30: 717–724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A., and Dismukes, A. (1995). Cardiovascular effects of experimentally induced efficacy (ability) appraisals at low and high levels of avoidant task demand.Psychophysiology, 32: 172–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sarah R. Baker.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baker, S.R., Stephenson, D. Prediction and control as determinants of behavioural uncertainty: Effects on task performance and heart rate reactivity. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science 35, 235–250 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02688786

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02688786

Keywords

Navigation