Skip to main content
Log in

On the partial independence of optimism and pessimism

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study tested the hypothesis that the moderate correlation between optimism (O) and pessimism (P) scores (their non-bipolarity) found in earlier studies may be an artifact of the differential sensitivity of O and P items to two response sets: defensive pessimism and Pollyannaism. The data failed to support either component of the hypothesis. Thus, defensive pessimists, as measured in two quite different ways, had lower depression scores (BDI) than genuine pessimists, but contrary to prediction, had higher depression scores than optimists. Moreover, deleting P items with a strong defensive quality from the P scale failed to increase the correlation between O and P scores. Similarly, controlling Pollyannaism by a partial correlation procedure failed to increase the strength of the relation between O and P. The results in general support the notion that optimism and pessimism are not polar opposites.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression.Psychological Review, 96, 358–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M.E.P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients in family practice: A rapid technique.Postgraduate Medicine, 52, 81–85.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Weissman, A., Lester, D., & Trexler, L. (1974). The measurement of pessimism: The hopelessness scale.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 861–865.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants.Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573–588.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cropley, A. J., & Weckowicz, T. E. (1966). The dimensionality of clinical depression.Australian Journal of Psychology, 18, 19–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dember, W. N., & Brooks, J. (1989). A new instrument for measuring optimism and pessimism: Test-retest reliability and relations with happiness and religious commitment.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 365–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dember, W. N., Martin, S. H., Hummer, M. K., Howe, S. R., & Melton, R. S. (1989). The measurement of optimism and pessimism.Current Psychological Research and Reviews, 8, 109–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dember, W. N., & Penwell, L. (1980). Happiness, depression, and the Pollyanna principle.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 321–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M., & Leitenberg, H. (1986). Optimism and pessimism in school-aged children.Child Development, 57, 241–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frauenfelder, K. J. (1974). A cognitive determinant of favorability of impression.Journal of Social Psychology, 94, 71–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gonen, J. Y. & Lansky, L. M. (1968). Masculinity, femininity, and masculinity-femininity: A phenomenological study of themf scale of the MMPI.Psychological Reports, 23, 183–194.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hummer, M. K. (1990).The differential vulnerability of optimism and pessimism to response sets. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamen, L. P., Rodin, J., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1987).Explanatory style and immune functioning. Unpublished manuscript, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanfer, F. H., & Schefft, B. K. (1988).Guiding the process of therapeutic change. Champaign, IL: Research Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kent, D. (1990). Optimists and pessimists, both have their place.American Psychological Society Observer, November, 18–19.

  • Kettelaer, T., & Sinnett, L. M. (1990). The relation between positive and negative affect and optimistic and pessimistic judgments: A mood congruency model. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Society, Dallas, TX.

  • Martin, S. H. (1986).Optimism, pessimism and psychological defense. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matlin, M. W., & Stang, D. J. (1978).The Pollyanna principle. Cambridge, MA: Shenkman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, M. K. (1983).The measurement of optimism/pessimism. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norem, J., & Cantor, N. (1984).Pessimism as a cognitive cushion. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.

  • Norem, J., & Cantor, N. (1986). Anticipatory and post-hoc cushioning strategies: Optimism and defensive pessimism in “risky” situations.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 103, 346–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. T., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957).The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., Seligman, M.E.P., & Vaillant, G. E. (1988). Pessimistic explanatory style as a risk factor for physical illness: A thirty-five-year longitudinal study.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 23–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & Villanova, P. (1988). An expanded attributional style questionnaire.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 87–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies.Health Psychology, 4, 219–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwab, M. D. (1984).Optimism, depression, and task expectancy. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Thorton, N., & Thornton, K. M. (1990). Explanatory style as a mechanism of disappointing athletic performance.Psychological Science, 1, 143–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978).Masculinity and femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terezis, H. C. (1990).Optimism and pessimism: Manipulation via musical mood induction. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winer, B. J. (1971).Statistical principles in experimental design (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zullow, H. M., Oettingen, G., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1988). Pessimistic explanatory style in the historical record: CAVing LBJ, presidential candidates, and East versus West Berlin.American Psychologist, 43, 673–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hummer, M.K., Dember, W.N., Melton, R.S. et al. On the partial independence of optimism and pessimism. Current Psychology 11, 37–50 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02686826

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02686826

Keywords

Navigation