Skip to main content
Log in

Reasons, cognition and society

  • Articles
  • Published:
Mind & Society Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Homo sociologicus and homo oeconomicus are, for different reasons, unsatisfactory models for the social sciences. A third model, called “rational model in the broad sense”, seems better endowed to cope with the many different expressions of rationality of the social agent. Some contributions by Weber, Durkheim and Marx are early examples of the application of this model of social explanation based on good subjective reasons. According to this model and to the evidence of cognitive anthropology, it is possible to reconcile primitive thinking with the inferential principles of Western people. Lastly, cognitive psychology can contribute to the discovery of generalizations of reason-based choices that can strengthen the explanatory power of “rational model in the broad sense”.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Berlin, B.O. & Kay, P.D. (1969)Basic Color Terms (Berkeley, University of California Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudon, R. (1981)The Logic of Social Action (London, Routledge).

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudon, R. (1994)The art of self-persuasion (London, Polity Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudon, R. (1997) What theory of rationality for the social sciences: the cognitivist model,Society and Economy, Quarterly Journal of the Budapest University of Economic Sciences 1, pp. 75–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Choi, S. & Bowerman, M. (1991) Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: the influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns,Cognition, 41, pp. 83–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, I., Nisbett, R.E. & Smith, E.E. (1997) Culture, Category Salience, and Inductive Reasoning,Cognition, 64, pp. 15–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1977) An ethnographic psychology of cognition, in P. Johnson-Laird & P.C. Wason (Eds.)Thinking (Cambridge, Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, A., Laudan, L., & Laudan, R. (1988)Scrutinizing Science (Dordrecht: Kluwer).

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim, E. (1897),Suicide, A Study in Sociology, (New York, The Free Press, 1962).

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim, E. (1912),Le formes élementaires de la vie religieuse (Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1979).

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J. (1985),Making sense of Marx (Cambridge, Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Pritchard, E.E. (1937),Witchcraft, Oracles and Magic among the Azande (Oxford, Clarendon).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gladwin, T. (1970)East is a Big Bird (Cambridge, Harvard University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P.M. & Childs, C. (1974) Weaving, color terms and patterns representation: cultural influences and cognitive development among Zinacantecos of southern Mexico, in J. Dawson & W. Lonner (Eds.)Reading in Cross-Cultural Psychology. Proceedings of the First International Conference of the International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology (Hong Kong, University of Hong Kong Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider, E. R. (1972) Universals in color naming and memory,Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, pp. 10–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, J., Payne, J.W. & Pluto, C. (1982) Adding asymmetrical dominated alternatives: violations of regularity and similarity hypothesis,Journal of Consumer Research, 9, pp. 90–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanheman, D. & Tversky, A. (1979) Prospect Theory: an analysis of decision under risk,Econometrica, 47, pp. 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S.C. (1994) Vision, shape and linguistic description: Tzeltal body part terminology and object description,Linguistics, 32, pp. 791–885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, A., Atran, S., Coley, J.D., Medin, D.L. & Smith, E.E. (1997) The Tree of Life: Universal and Cultural Features of Folkbiological Taxonomies and Inductions,Cognitive Psychology, 32, pp. 251–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, B. (1954)Magic, Science and Religion (New York: Doubleday).

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx, K. (1867)Das Kapital (Hamburg, O. Meissner; New York, L. W. Schmidt).

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D.L., Lynch, E., Coley, J.D. & Atran, S. (1977) Categorization and reasoning among tree experts: Do all roads lead to Rome?,Cognitive Psychology, 32, pp. 49–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myrvold, W.C. (1996) Bayesianism and Diverse Evidence: A Reply to Andrew Wayne,Philosophy of Science, 63, pp. 661–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. (1965)The Logic of Collective Action (Cambridge, Harvard University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Osherson, D., Smith, E.E., Wilkie, O., Lopez, A. & Shafir, E. (1990) Category-based induction,Psychological Review, 97, pp. 185–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osherson, D. & Viale, R. (1999) The difference phenomenon and the little scientist hypothesis. in L. Magnani, N. Nersessian & P. Thagard (Eds.)Model Base Reasoning in Scientific Discovery. Proceedings of MBR '98 Conference, forthcoming.

  • Rosch, E. (1972) The Structure of Color Space in Naming and Memory for Two Languages,Cognitive Psychology, 3, pp. 337–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapir, E. (1993) Choosing versus rejecting: why some options are both better and worse than others,Memory & Cognition, 21, pp. 546–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapir, E., Simonson, I. & Tversky, A. (1993) Reason-based choice,Cognition, 49, pp. 11–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R.A. (1977) Likeness and likelihood in everyday thought: magical thinking and everyday judgments about personality, in P.N. Johnson-Laird & P.C. Wason (Eds.)Thinking (Cambridge, Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1982),Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge, MIT Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonson, I. & Tversky, A. (1992) Choice in context: tradeoff contrast and extremeness aversion,Journal of Marketing Research, 29, pp. 281–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tambiah, S.J. (1973) Form and meaning of magical acts: a point of view, in R. Horton & R. Finnegan (Eds.)Models of Thought (London, Faber and Faber).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. & Kanheman, D. (1974) Judgment under uncertainty heuristics and biases,Science, 185, pp. 1124–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viale, R., & Osherson, D. (submitted). Category-based reasoning in Vietnam and Italy.

  • von Neumann, J. & Morgenstern, O. (1947)Theory of games and economic behavior (Princeton, Princeton University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. (1922)Wirtschaft und Gesellschaft (Tubingen, Mohr).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Riccardo Viale.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Boudon, R., Viale, R. Reasons, cognition and society. Mind & Society 1, 41–56 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02512228

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02512228

Keywords

Navigation