Skip to main content
Log in

The relevance of a connectionist model of reading for “The great debate”

  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Connectionist models of reading, in particular Seidenberg and McClelland's (1989) parallel distributed processing model of word recognition, are described and contrasted with the top-down, bottom-up, and interactive models of reading from the traditional, symbol-manipulation paradigm of information processing. Connectionism emphasizes: (1) a single, rather than dual, mechanism for processing words, and (2) distributed representations and weighted connections between units rather than symbolic rules for mapping letters and sounds. Implications for the teaching of reading are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990).Beginning to Read, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J., and Strawson, C. (1976). Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud.J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 2: 386–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W., and Abrahamsen, D. (1991).Connectionism and the Mind, Backwell, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C. (1991). Implications of connectionism for thinking about rules.Educ. Res. 20: 10–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. E. (1979). The independence of reading and spelling in backward and normal readers.Devel. Med. Child Neurol. 21: 504–514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. E. (1985).Rhyme and Reason in Reading and Spelling, University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor, MI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryk, A. S., and Raudenbush, S. W. (1992).Hierarchical Linear Models, Sage Publications, Newbury Park, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. (1992). Studies in the acquisition procedure for reading: Rationale, hypotheses, and data. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., and Treiman, R. (eds.),Reading Acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cazden, C. (1992).Whole Language Plus, Teachers College Press, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. (1967).Learning to Read: The Great Debate, McGraw-Hill, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. (1983).Stages of Reading Development, McGraw-Hill, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clay, M. (1985).The Early Detection of Reading Difficulties (3rd Ed.), Heinemann Educational Books. Portsmouth, NH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clymer, T. (1963). The utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades.Reading Teacher 16: 252–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In Underwood, G. (ed.),Strategies of Information Processing, Academic Press, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., and Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches.Psychol. Rev. 100: 589–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeFord, D. E., Lyons, C. A., and Pinnell, G. S. (1991) (eds.)Bridges to Literacy: Learning from Reading Recovery, Heinemann, Portsmouth, NH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1991).Consciousness Explained, Little, Brown & Co., Boston, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (in press). Development of the ability to read words: Update. In Ruddell, R., Ruddell, M., and Singer, H. (eds.),Theoretical Models and Processes of Reading. IRA, Newark, Delaware.

  • Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., and Treiman, R. (eds.),Reading Acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 107–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Englemann, S., and Bruner, E. C. (1983).Reading Mastery I and II: DISTAR Reading, Science Research Associates, Chicago, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In Pattern, K. E., Marshall, J. C., and Coltheart, M. (eds.),Surface Dyslexia, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 301–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Novy, D. M., and Liberman, D. (1991). How letter-sound instruction mediates progress in first-grade reading and spelling.J. Educ. Psychol. 83: 456–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Jenkins, L., and Francis, D. J. (1993). Links among segmenting, spelling, and reading words in first and second grades.Reading Writing 5: 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., and Levin, H. (1975).The Psychology of Reading, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud.J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 5: 674–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, K. S. (1970). Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game. In Singer, H., and Ruddell, R. B. (eds.),Theoretical Models and Processes of Reading, International Reading Association, Newark, DE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1986). Children's use of analogy in learning to read: A developmental study.J. Exp. Child Psychol. 42: 73–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., and Bryant, P. E. (1990).Phonological Skills and Learning to Read, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. (1972). One second of reading. In Kavanagh, J. F., and Mattingly, I. G. (eds.),Language by Ear and by Eye, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 331–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., and Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Learning to read: An unnatural act.Bull. Orton Soc. 30: 179–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., and Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability.Remed. Spec. Educ. 7: 6–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., Juel, C., and Griffith, P. (1992). Reading, spelling, and the orthographic cipher. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., and Treiman, R. (eds.),Reading Acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 35–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haskell, D. W., Foorman, B. R., and Swank, P. (1992). Effects of three orthographic/phonological units on first-grade reading.Remed. Spec. Educ. 13: 40–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, G. E., and Sallice, T. (1989). Lesioning a Connectionist Network: Investigations of Acquired Dyslexia. Technical Report CRG-TR-89-3, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K. J. (1991). Symbolic connectionism: Toward third-generation theories of expertise. In Ericsson, K. A., and Smith, J. (eds.),Toward a General Theory of Expertise, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1990). Beginning reading. In Barr, R., Kamil, M. L., Mosenthal, P., and Pearson, R. D. (eds.),Handbook of Reading Research, (Vol. 2), Longman, New York, pp. 759–788.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. W., and Bertoncini, J. (1988). Viewing the development of speech perception as an innately guided learning process.Lang. Speech 31: 217–238.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., and Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension.Psychol. Rev. 87: 329–354.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, Y., and Bryant, P. E. (1983). Reading and writing in English and Japanese.Brit. J. Devel. Psychol. 1: 129–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, H., and Francis, W. N. (1967).Computational Analysis of Present-Day American English, Brown University Press, Providence, RI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Frost, J., and Peterson, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in pre-school children.Reading Res. Quart. 23: 263–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B., Leinbach, J., Taraban, R., and McDonald, J. (1989). Language learning: Cues or rules?J. Memory Lang. 28: 255–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1, An account of basic findings.Psychol. Rev. 88: 375–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985).Reading Ability, Oxford University Press, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1991). Representations and awareness in the acquisition of reading competence. In Rieben, L., and Perfetti, C. A. (eds.),Learning to Read: Basic Research and Its Implications, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L., and Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children.Merrill-Palmer Quart. 33: 283–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitto, L. A. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language.Cognition 27: 1–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petitto, L. A., and Marentette, P. F. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language.Science 251: 1493–1496.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., and Mehler, J. (eds.) (1988). Connectionism and symbol systems: Special edition.Cognition 28.

  • Pinker, S., and Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition.Cognition 28: 73–194.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., and Pollatsek, A. (1989).The Psychology of Reading, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Toward an interactive model of reading. In Dornic, S. (ed.),Attention and Performance VI, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 573–603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1989). The architecture of mind: A connectionist approach. In Posner, M. I. (ed.),Foundations of Cognitive Science, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 133–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., and McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. (eds.),Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition (Vol. 2), MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 216–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E., and Williams, R. J. (1986). Learning internal representations by error propagation. In Rumelhart, D. E., and McClelland, J. L. (eds.),Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition (Vol. 1), MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 318–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., and Graham, D. J. (1992). Introduction to connectionist modeling in education.Educ. Psychol. 27: 513–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. (1990). Lexical access: Another theoretical soupstone? In Balota, D. A., Flores D'Arcais, G. B., and Rayner, K. (eds.),Comprehension Processes in Reading, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 33–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. (1992). Dyslexia in a computational model of word recognition in reading. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., and Treiman, R. (eds.),Reading Acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 243–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M., and McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming.Psychol. Rev. 96: 523–568.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., Bruck, M., Fornarolo, G., and Backman, J. (1986). Word recognition skills of poor and disabled readers: Do they necessarily differ?Appl. Psycholing. 6: 161–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sejnowski, T. J., and Rosenberg, C. R. (1987). Parallel networks that learn to pronounce English test.Complex Syst. 1: 145–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F. (1971).Understanding Reading: A Psycholinguistic Analysis of Reading and Learning to Read, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soderbergh, R. (1971).Reading in Early Childhood: A Linguistic Study of a Swedish Preschool Child's Gradual Acquisition of Reading Ability, Almgvist and Wiksell, Stockholm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1990). A call for an end to the paradigm wars in reading research.J. Reading Behav. 22: 221–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In Gough, P. B., Ehri, L. C., and Treiman, R. (eds.),Reading Acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 307–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, M., and Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequences of stages?Cognition 30: 139–181.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. A., Freyd, J. J., and Baron, J. (1983). Phonological recoding and use of spelling sound rules in reading of sentences.J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 22: 682–700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B. F., and Stone, G. O. (1990). Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics.Psychol. Rev. 97: 488–522.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R. (1991). Introduction to three studies on reading acquisition: Convergent findings on theoretical foundations of code-oriented versus whole-language approaches to reading instruction.J. Educ. Psychol. 83: 437–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., and Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills.Psychol. Bull. 101: 192–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1969). Context-sensitive coding, associative memory, and serial order in (speech) behavior.Psychol. Rev. 76: 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wise, B. W. (1992). Whole words and decoding for short-term learning: Comparisons on a “talking-computer” system.J. Exp. Child Psychol. 54: 147–167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Foorman, B.R. The relevance of a connectionist model of reading for “The great debate”. Educ Psychol Rev 6, 25–47 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02209023

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02209023

Key Words

Navigation