Skip to main content
Log in

Analysis of behavioral traits in the presence of cultural transmission and assortative mating: Applications to IQ and SES

  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

General linear models of familial resemblance are described which allow for polygenic inheritance, cultural transmission from parent to offspring, phenotypic assortative mating, common environment, and maternal and parental effects. These models use observed phenotypic correlations between multiple classes of relatives and/or correlations between individuals reared in separated and extended family structures to yield maximum likelihood parameter estimates. The models are first applied to American kinship data for IQ, with the variance of IQ partitioned as 30% additive genetic, 29% due to cultural inheritance, 9% due to gene-culture covariance, and 32% due to nontransmissible environment. Under the assumption that the correlations between (nontransmissible) environments of DZ and MZ twins are the same, an approximate treatment of dominance yields an estimate of 23% dominance variation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Burt, C. (1966). The genetic determination of differences in intelligence: A study of monozygotic twins reared together and apart.Br. J. Psychol. 57:137–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. (1973). Models for cultural inheritance. I. Group mean and within group variation.Theor. Pop. Biol. 4:47–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. (1978). The evolution of continuous variations. III. Joint transmission of genotypic phenotype and environment.Genetics 90:391–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. (1979). resolution of polygenic and cultural inheritance from reciprocal phenotype-environment interaction. I. Introduction and description of the general dynamic model. Submitted.

  • Cloninger, C. R. and Rice, J. 1979. Resolution of polygenic and cultural inheritance from reciprocal phenotype-environment interaction. III. Familial resemblance and the use of multiple correlated traits in identifying causal factors. Submitted.

  • Cloninger, C. R., Rice, J., and Reich, T. (1979a). Multifactorial inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating. II. A general model of combined polygenic and cultural inheritance.Am. J. Hum. Genet. 31:176–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R., Rice, J., and Reich, T. (1979b). Multifactorial inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating. III. Family structure and the analysis of separation experiments.Am. J. Hum. Genet. 31:366–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R. Rice, J., Rao, D. C. and Morton, N. E. (1979c). Resolution of polygenic and cultural inheritance from reciprocal phenotype-environment interaction II. Resemblance between mates due to mixed structural relations. Submitted.

  • Dorfman, D. D. (1978). The Cyril Burt question: New findings.Science 201:1177–1186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, O. D., and Featherman, D. L. (1973). Psychological and cultural factors in the process of occupational achievement. In Goldberger, A. S., and Duncan, O. D., (eds.),Structural Equation Models in the Social Sciences, Seminar Press, New York. pp. 229–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, O. D., Featherman, D. L., and Duncan, B. (1972).Socioeconomic Background and Achievement, Seminar Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, L. J. (1976). The effect of cultural transmission on continuous variation.Heredity 34:41–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, M. W., and Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1977). The evolution of continuous variation. II. Complex transmission and assortative mating.Theor. Pop., Biol. 11:161–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, M. W., and Lewontin, R. C. (1975). The heritability hang-up.Science 190:1163–1168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. A. (1918). The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian inheritance.Tr. R. Soc. Edinburgh 52:399–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberger, A. S. (1978). Models and methods in the IQ debate: Part I (Revised). SSRI Workshop Paper 7801, Social Systems Research Institute, University of Wisconsin, Madison.

  • Hearnshaw, L. S. (1979).Cyril Burt, Psychologist, Cornell University Press, Ithaca, N. Y.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. L. (1973).I.Q. in the Meritocracy, Atlantic Monthly Press, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jencks, C. (1972).Inequality: A Reassessment of the Effect of Family and Schooling in America, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamin, L. (1974).The Science and Politics of IQ, Halstead Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kempthorne, O. (1978). Logical, epistemological and statistical aspects of nature-nuture data interpretations.Biometrics 34:1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, M. G. and Stuart, A. (1973).The Advanced Theory of Statistics, Vol. 2:Inference and Relationship, 3rd ed., Hafner, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leahy, A. (1935). Nature nuture and intelligence.Genet. Psychol Monogr. 17:241–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J. C. (1978). Heredity-environment analyses of Jencks's IQ correlations.Behav. Genet. 8:415–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, N. E. (1974). Analysis of family resemblance. I. Introduction.Am. J. Hum. Genet. 26:318–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, N. H., Freeman, F. N., and Holzinger, K. J. (1937).Twins: A Study of Heredity and Environment, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rao, D. C., and Morton, N. E. (1978). IQ as a paradigm in genetic epidemiology. In Morton, N., and Chung, C. S., (eds.),Genetic Epidemiology, Academic Press, New York, pp. 145–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rao, D. C., Morton, N. E., and Yee, S. (1974). Analysis of family resemblance. II. A linear model for familial correlation.Am. J. Hum. Genet. 26:331–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rao, D. C., Morton, N. E., and Yee, S. (1976). Resolution of cultural and biological inheritance by path analysis.Am. J. Hum. Genet. 28:228–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rao, D. C., Morton, N. E., and Cloninger, C. R. (1979). Path Analysis under generalized assortative mating. I. Theory.Genet. Res. 33:175–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, J., Cloninger, C. R., and Reich, T. (1978). Multifactorial Inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating. I. Description and basic properties of the unitary models.Am. J. Hum. Genet. 30:618–643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, D., and Polmin, R. (1978). The Burt controversy: A comparison of Burt's data on IQ with data from other studies.Behav. Genet. 8:81–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taubman, P. (1977).Kinometrics: Determinnts of Socioeconomic Success Within and Between Families, North-Holland, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wade, N. (1976). IQ and heredity: Suspicion of traud beclouds classic experiment.Science 194:916–919.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (1931). Statistical methods in biology.Proc. Am. Stat. Assoc. 26:155–163.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This work was supported in part by USPHS Grants AA-03539, MH-07081, MH-25430, and MH-31302 and by Research Scientist Development Award MH-00048 (C. R. C.).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rice, J., Cloninger, C.R. & Reich, T. Analysis of behavioral traits in the presence of cultural transmission and assortative mating: Applications to IQ and SES. Behav Genet 10, 73–92 (1980). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067320

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067320

Key Words

Navigation