Skip to main content
Log in

Intonation and meaning

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A general theory of meaning is introduced, based on a distinction between the content of a mental act and its mode of arousal. The distinction is extended to speech acts and to the role of intonation in determining their mode of utterance. Difference judgments based on intonation are used to produce a mode space with dimensions of Credence, Option, Salience, Arousal, and Weak Belief. It is argued that the modal function of intonations should not be restricted to the enrichment of declarations. The distinction between mode and content is used to separate the specficially linguistic content of intonations from other contents which they convey perceptually and by implication.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bloomfield, L. (1935).Language, Allen & Unwin, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, D. (ed.), (1972a).Intonation, Penguin Books, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, D. (1972b). Relative height. In Bolinger, D. (ed.),Intonation, Penguin Books, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, N. T. (1972). Tones and intonations in the Chengtu dialect. In Bolinger, D. (ed.),Intonation, Penguin Books, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crystal, D. (1969).Prosodic Systems and Intonation in English, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denes, P. (1959). Preliminary investigation of certain aspects of intonation.Lang. Spech 2:106–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1969).Content and Consciousness, Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., and Raven, B. H. (1967). The AB scales: An operational definition of belief and attitude. In Fishbein, M. (ed.),Readings in Attitude Theory and Measurement, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K., and Scheerer, M. (1941). Abstract and concrete behavior.Psychol. Monogr. 53:2 (Whole No. 239).

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1966). Intonation systems in English. In Halliday M. A. K., and McIntosh, A. (eds.),Patterns of Language, Longmans, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1884). What is an emotion.Mind 9:188–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kneale, W. (1968). Intentionality and intensionality.Proc. Aristotelian Soc. 42:73–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruskal, J. B. (1964a). Multidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of fit.Psychometrika,29:1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruskal, J. B. (1964b). Non-metric multidimensional scaling: A numerical method.Psychometrika,29:115–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P., and Michaels, S. B. (1962). Some aspects of fundamental frequency and envelope amplitude as related to the emotional content of speech.J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 23:922–927.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (1960). A syllogistic analysis of cognitive relatonships. In Rosenberg, N. J., et al. (eds.),Attitude Organization and Change, Harvard University Press, New Haven.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Connor, J. D. (1973).Phonetics, Pelican Books, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pike, K. L. (1972). General characteristics of intonation. In Bolinger, D. (ed.),Intonation, Penguin Books, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozeboom, W. W. (1972). Problems in the psycho-philosophy of knowledge. In Royce, J. R., and Rozeboom, W. W. (eds.),The Psychology of Knowing, Gordon & Breach, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartre, J. P. (1962).Sketch for a Theory of the Emotions, Methuen, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1972). Conceptual dependency: A theory of natural language understanding.Cogn. Psychol. 3:552–631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1973). The fourteen primitive actions and their inferences.Stanford Artificial Intelligence Memo, AIM-183.

  • Searle, J. R. (1970).Speech Acts, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. E. (1968). The bothersomeness of inconsistency. In Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., et al. (eds.),Theories of Cognitive Consistency: A Sourcebook, Rand McNally, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tesler, L., Enea, H., and Colby, K. M. (1968). A directed graph representation for computer simulation of belief-systems.Math. Biosci. 21:2–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tonkova-Yampol'skaya, R. V. (1973). Development of speech intonation in infants during the first two years of life. In Ferguson, C. A., and Slobin, D. I. (eds.),Studies of Child Language Development, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uldall, E. (1972). Dimensions of meaning in intonation. In Bolinger, D. (ed.),Intonation, Penguin Books, London.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was supported by a Canada Council Doctoral Fellowship) (W70 3030, W71 4373).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Egan, O. Intonation and meaning. J Psycholinguist Res 9, 23–39 (1980). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067300

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067300

Keywords

Navigation