Skip to main content
Log in

Evidence for the role of the justice motive in status generalization processes

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Status generalization refers to the capacity of status characteristics, such as race, gender, or occupation, to become the basis of social inequality even when such characteristics are not directly relevant to the immediate social interaction. For instance, individuals with positively valued status characteristics are more likely to assume positions of power and prestige than those of lower standing. To date, status generalization has been assumed to arise from stereotyped beliefs that associate positive standing on a status characteristic with positively valued personal attributes. Findings from two studies indicate that the capacity for status characteristics to produce power and prestige orders may also depend on people's need to believe in a just world. In the first study, participants were fortuitously granted a position of either superior or equal power and prestige to another person. Consistent with just world predictions, participants portrayed themselves more positively relative to the other on assessments of personal attributes when they occupied a superior position than when they occupied a position of equal power and prestige. Results from a second study showed that the tendency to link personal attributes with power and prestige standing was strongest for participants who scored high on the Just World Scale. Thus, just world beliefs appear to play a role in status generalization processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aronson, E. (1978). The theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.),Cognitive Theories in Social Psychology, Academic Press, New York, pp. 215–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, W., and Hatfield, E. (1980). Equity theory, power, and social justice. In Mikula, G. (ed.),Justice and Social Interaction: Experimental and Theoretical Contributions from Psychological Research, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 25–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Cohen, B. P., and Zelditch, M., Jr. (1972). Status characteristics and social interaction.Am. Sociol. Rev. 37: 241–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Fisek, M. H., Norman, R. Z., and Zelditch, M. (1977).Status Characteristics and Social Interaction: An Expectations-States Approach, Elsevier, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Rosenholtz, S. J., and Zelditch, M., Jr. (1980). Status organizing organizing processes.Ann. Rev. Sociol. 6: 479–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blau, P. (1964).Exchange and Power in Social Life, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. L. (1986). Power and justice in intergroup relations. In Bierhoff, H. W., Cohen, R. L., and Greenberg, J. (eds.),Justice in Social Relations, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 65–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, K. S., and Hegtvedt, K. A. (1986). Justice and power: An exchange analysis. In Bierhoff, H. W., Cohen, R. L., and Greenberg, J. (eds.),Justice in Social Relations, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 19–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Della Fave, L. R. (1980). The meek shall not inherit the earth: Self-evaluation and the legitimacy of stratification.Am. Sociol. Rev. 45: 955–971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dion, K. L., and Dion, K. K. (1987). Belief in a just world and physical attractiveness stereotyping.J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52: 775–780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driskell, J. E. (1982). Personal characteristics and performance expectations.Soc. Psychol. Quart. 45: 229–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knottnerus, J. D. (1988). A critique of expectation states theory: Theoretical assumptions and models of social cognition.Social. Perspect. 31: 420–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J., and Benevento, A. (1978). Self-induced dependence.J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 36: 886–893.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasswell, H. D., and Lerner, D. (1966).World Revolutionary Elites, M.I.T. Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, M. L., and Ofshe, R. (1981). The impact of behavioural style and status characteristics on social influence: A test of two competing theories.Soc. Psychol. Quart. 44: 73–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenski, G. E. (1967). Status inconsistency and the vote: A four nations test.Am. Sociol. Rev. 32: 298–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, M. J. (1965). Evaluation of performance as a function of performer's reward and attractiveness.J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1: 355–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, M. J. (1980).The Belief in a Just World: A Fundamental Delusion, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, M. J., and Simmons, C. H. (1966). The observer's reaction to the “innocent victim”: Compassion or rejection?J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 4: 203–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., and Ross, L. (1980).Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L., and Berger, J. (1986). Expectations, legitimation, and dominance behavior in task groups.Am. Sociol. Rev. 51: 603–617.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, Z., and Peplau, L. A. (1975). Who believes in a just world?J. Soc. Issues 31: 65–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, E. E. (1969). Studies of status congruence. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.),Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 4, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sande, G. N., Ellard, J. H., and Ross, M. (1986). Effect of arbitrarily assigned status labels on self-perceptions and social perceptions: The mere position effect.J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50: 684–689.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker, B. R. (1980).Impression Management: The Self-Concept, Social Indentity, and Interpersonal Relations, Brooks/Cole, Monterey, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephenson, G. M., and White, J. H. (1968). An experimental study of some effects of injustice on children's moral behaviour.J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 4: 460–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephenson, G. M., and White, J. H. (1970). Privilege, deprivation, and children's moral behaviour: An experimental clarification of the role of investments.J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 6: 167–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strodtbeck, F. L., James, R. J., and Hawkins, C. (1957). Social status in jury deliberations.Am. Sociol. Rev. 22: 713–719.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E. (1981). The interface of cognitive and social psychology. In Hamilton, D. L. (ed.),Cognitive Processes in Stereotyping and Intergroup Behavior, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 189–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster, M. Jr., and Driskell, J. E. (1978). Status generalization: A review and some new data.Am. Sociol. Rev. 43: 220–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster, M. Jr., and Driskell, J. E. (1983). Beauty as status.Am. J. Sociol. 89: 141.165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winer, B. J. (1971).Statistical Principles in Experimental Design, McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ellard, J.H., Bates, D.D. Evidence for the role of the justice motive in status generalization processes. Soc Just Res 4, 115–134 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01053242

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01053242

Key words

Navigation