Skip to main content
Log in

Causal attribution and judicial discretion

A look at the verbal behavior of municipal court judges

  • Articles
  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

Abstract

An important need of the criminal justice system is to better understand and structure discretionary decisions, thereby minimizing their adverse effects. The present study explored the relevance of models of attribution processes to these goals by analysing judges' verbal statements in real courtroom settings. Results reveal that these models are useful in studying discretion and that the method is a fruitful component of a multimethodological approach to studying both attribution and discretion. The importance of both developing and testing models in real life contexts was stressed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Reference Note

  • Dorch, E. Prosecutorial discretion. Unpublished M.A. thesis, University of Missouri, Kansas City, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

References

  • Allport, G. W.The Nature of Prejudice. Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley, 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bermant, G., McGuire, M., McKinley, W., and Salo, C. The logic of simulation in jury research.Criminal Justice and Behavior, 1974,1(3), 221–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. S., & Payne, J. W. Judgements about crime and the criminal: A model and a method for investigating parole decisions. In B. D. Sales (Ed.),Perspectives in Law and Psychology, Vol. 1: The Criminal Justice System. New York: Plenum, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, K. C.Discretionary Justice: A Preliminary Inquiry. Baton Rouge, Louisiana: Louisiana State University Press, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebbesen, E. B., & Konechi, V. J. Decision making and information integration in the courts: the setting of bail.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975,32, 805–821.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, G. Social comparison and some determinants of expected personal control and expected performance in a novel task situation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1974,29, 487–496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, G. Causal attribution in simulated versus real situations: When are people logical, when are they not?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975,32, 1021–1029.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, G., & Kiger, R. The effects of defendant dress and supervision on judgments of simulated jurors.Law and Human Behavior, 1978,2, 75–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frieze, I., & Weiner, B. Cue utilization and attributional judgments for success and failure.Journal of Personality, 1971,39, 591–605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, P. L. Cognitive biases in the perception of social groups. In J. S. Carroll and J. W. Payne (Eds.),Cognition and Social Behavior. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F.The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New York: Wiley, 1958.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. From acts to dispositions. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & McGillis, D. Correspondent inferences and the attribution cube: A comparative reappraisal. In J. H. Harvey et al. (Eds.),New Directions in Attribution Research, Vol. 1. New York: Wiley, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.),Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. The process of causal attribution.American Psychologist, 1973,28, 107–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippman, W.Public Opinion. New York: Harcourt Brace, 1922.

    Google Scholar 

  • McArthur, L. A. The how and what of why: some determinants and consequences of causal attribution.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1972,22, 171–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Advisory Commission on Criminal Justice Standards and Goals,Courts. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A.Human Problem Solving. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W. Braunstein, M. L., & Carroll, J. S. Exploring predecisional behavior: an alternative approach to decision research.Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 1978,22, 17–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaver, K. G., Gilbert, M. A., & Williams, M. D. Social psychology criminal justice and the principle of discretion: a selective review.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1955,1, 471–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M. L., Stephan, W. G., & Rosenfield, D. Attributional egotism. In J. H. Harvey et al. (Eds.),New Directions in Attribution Research, Vol. 1. New York: Wiley, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, C. Selectivity characteristics of jurors and litigants: their influences on juries' verdicts. In R. J. Simon (Ed.),The Jury System in America: A Critical Overview, Beverly Hills, California: Sage, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., Heckhausen, H., Meyer, W., & Cook, R. E. Causal ascriptions and achievement behavior: conceptual analysis of effort and reanalysis of locus of control.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1972,21, 239–248.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The authors thank Janice Bryan and Patsi Roberts for their help in data collection.

About this article

Cite this article

Fontaine, G., Emily, C. Causal attribution and judicial discretion. Law Hum Behav 2, 323–337 (1978). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01038985

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01038985

Keywords

Navigation