Skip to main content
Log in

Perceptions of similarity between striving tasks and the generality of task preferences

  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Ninth graders rated their degree of liking for each of 12 striving tasks and, on another occasion, rated their perceptions of the similarity between each pair of tasks. Analyses of variance revealed strong PersonX Task interactions, suggesting the possibility of individual differences in task preference generality. A derived measure of task preference generality-specificity related significantly to an overall measure of perceived task similarity—“generalizers” saw the tasks as being more similar than did “specifiers.” Investigation of individual differences in degree of cross-situational generality was suggested as a way to integrate “trait” and “situational” theories of behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport, G. W.Personality. A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt, 1937.

    Google Scholar 

  • Attneave, F. Dimensions of similarity.American Journal of Psychology 1950,63 516–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H.Social learning and personality development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. On predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior.Psychological Review 1974,81 506–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunswik, E.Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton, R. V. Generality of honesty reconsidered.Psychological Review 1963,70 481–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campus, N. Trans-situational consistency as a dimension of personality,Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1974,29 593–600.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. D., & Chang, J. J. Analysis of individual differences in multi-dimensional scaling via an N-way generalization of “Eckart-Young” decomposition.Psychometrika 1970,35 283–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cottrell, L. S., Jr. The analysis of situational fields in social psychology.American Sociological Review 1942,7 370–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coutu, W.Emergent human nature. New York: Knopf, 1949.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crandall, V. C.Parents' influences on children's achievement behavior. Progress report, USPHS grant no. MH-02238. Fels Institute, Yellow Springs, Ohio, 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crandall, V. C., & Battle, E. S. The antecedents and adult correlates of academic and intellectual achievement effort. In J. P. Hill (Ed.),Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 4). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1970, Pp. 36–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodd, D. H., & Schultz, R. F., Jr. Computational procedures for estimating magnitude of effect for some analysis of variance designs.Psychological Bulletin 1973,79 391–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekehammar, B. Interactionism in personality from a historical perspective.Psychological Bulletin 1974,81 1026–1048.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, N. S., & Hunt, J. McV. S-R inventories of hostility and comparisons of the proportions of variance from persons, responses, and situations for hostility and anxiousness.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1968,9 309–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, N. S., & Hunt, J. McV. Generalizability of contributions from sources of variance in the S-R inventories of anxiousness.Journal of Personality 1969,37 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, R. W., & Schoen, R. A. Differentiation and abstraction in concept formation.Psychological Monographys, 1962,76 (41, Whole No. 560).

  • Hammond, K. R. Representative vs. systematic design in clinical psychology.Psychological Bulletin 1954,51 150–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartshorne, H., & May, M. A.Studies in the nature of character. Vol. 1: Studies in deceit. New York: Macmillan, 1928.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, G. A.The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton, 1955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kogan, N., & Wallach, M.Risk taking: A study in cognition and personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K.Dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W.Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality.Psychological Review 1973,80 252–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, E. A., Grinder, R. E., & Mutterer, M. L. Sources of variance in behavioral measures of honesty in temptation situations: Methodological analyses.Developmental Psychology 1969,1 265–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J. B.Social learning and clinical psychology. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, W. A. Cognitive complexity and cognitive flexibility.Sociometry 1962,25 405–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sermat, V. Is game behavior related to behavior in other interpersonal situations?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1970,16 92–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P. Information processing, situation specificity, and the generality of risk-taking behavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1972,22 128–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, D. The generality of children's achievement-related behavior.Journal of Genetic Psychology 1969,114 109–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vannoy, J. S. Generality of cognitive complexity-simplicity as a personality construct.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1965,2 385–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, M. A. On psychological similarity.Psychological Review 1958,65 103–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winer, B. J.Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Data collection and initial data analyses for this study were conducted under the auspices of the Institute for Juvenile Research, Chicago, Illinois. Gratitude is expressed for the cooperation of the staffs and students at St. Michaels and Immaculata high schools, Chicago; for useful discussions of theoretical issues with Mark Oberlander; and for much assistance with data analysis provided by Arthur Kendall.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Solomon, D. Perceptions of similarity between striving tasks and the generality of task preferences. Motiv Emot 1, 181–192 (1977). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00998519

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00998519

Keywords

Navigation