Skip to main content
Log in

Choosing between micro and macro nonverbal measurement: Application to selected vocalic and kinesic indices

  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Selecting the appropriate size of measurement unit in nonverbal research requires empirical as well as conceptual analysis. A definition is offered to distinguish macroscopic from microscopic measurement. Merits of each measurement approach are reviewed briefly in terms of cost, efficiency, precision, and analysis flexibility, and four additional criteria are advocated as central considerations in choosing measurement units: (1) isomorphism between the form of measurement and the phenomenological experience of interactants, (2) reliability, (3) concurrent validity, and (4) predictive validity. These criteria are applied to the analysis of 20 vocalic and kinesic nonverbal behaviors measured microscopically and macroscopically. Reliability and validity results demonstrate that either approach may be acceptable for many behaviors, but that relatively molecular coding may be preferable for highly dynamic and “objective” behaviors, while relatively molar coding may be preferable for static behaviors and perceptual judgments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1986).Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boice, R. & Monti, P. M. (1982). Specification of nonverbal behaviors for clinical assessment.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 7 79–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baesler, E. J. & Burgoon, J. K. (1987). Measurement and reliability of nonverbal behavior.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11 205–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 243–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birdwhistell, R. L. (1957).Kinesics and context: Essays on body motion communication. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunswik, E. (1956).Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R., Baron, R., Goodman, N., & Shapiro, B. (1980). Unitization of spontaneous nonverbal behavior in the study of emotion communication.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 522–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B. & Woodall, G. W. (1989).Nonverbal communication: The unspoken dialogue. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Olney, C. A., & Coker, R. A. (1987). The effects of communicator characteristics on patterns of reciprocity and compensation.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11 146–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Kelley, D. L., Newton, D. A., & Keeley-Dyreson, M. P. (1989). The nature of arousal and nonverbal indices.Human Communication Research, 16 217–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Newton, D. A., Walther, J. B., & Baesler, E. J. (1989). Nonverbal expectancy violations and conversational involvement.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13 97–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., & Saine, T. (1978).The unspoken dialogue. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N. (1981). Mutual influence in expressive behavior: Adult-adult and infant-adult dyadic interaction.Psychological Bulletin, 89 101–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N. (1983). Conversational involvement: Approaching and avoiding others. In. J. M. Wiemann & R. P. Harrison (Eds.),Nonverbal interaction (pp. 113–148). Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N. (1984). The relevance of the microstructure of interaction to relationship change.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1 239–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N. (1991). Mutual adaptation and relativity of measurement. In B. M. Montgomery & S. Duck (Eds.),Studying interpersonal interaction (pp. 103–117). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N. & Street, R. L., Jr. (1989). Message effects: Theory and research on mental models of messages. In J. J. Bradac (Ed.),Message effects in communication science (pp. 24–51). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coker, D. A. & Burgoon, J. K. (1987). The nature of conversational involvement and nonverbal encoding patterns.Human Communication Research, 13 463–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1982). Methods in measuring facial action. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.),Handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research (pp. 45–90). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. (1989). Remarks to the Iowa Conference on Personal Relationships, May 14, 1989. Reprinted inIowa/International Network Newsletter, 8, 7–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Hippel, W. (1990).The effects of schemata on memory for instances. Unpublished dissertation: University of Michigan.

  • Honeycutt, J. M. (1991). The role of nonverbal behavior in modifying expectancies during initial encounters.The Southern Communication Journal, 56 161–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowall, J. J. (1978). Interactional synchrony: A reappraisal.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9 963–975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newtson, D. (1973). Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28 28–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newtson, D., Engquist, G., Bois, J. (1977). The objective basis of behavior units.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 847–862.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1983).Nonverbal behavior: A functional perspective. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philpott, J. S. (1983).The relative contribution to meaning of verbal and nonverbal channels of communication: A meta-analysis. Master's thesis: University of Nebraska.

  • Scherer, K. R. (1978). Personality inference rules in personality attribution from voice quality: The loud voice of extroversion.European Journal of Social Psychology, 8 467–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Street, R. L., Jr. (1988). Communication style: Considerations for measuring consistency, reciprocity, and compensation. In C. H. Tardy (Ed.),A handbook for the study of human communication: Methods and instruments for observing, measuring, and assessing human communication processes (pp. 139–161). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Street, R. L., Jr., & Buller, D. B. (1987). Nonverbal response patterns in physician-patient interactions: A functional analysis.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11 234–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn, G. L. (1973). Cognitive integration of verbal and vocal information in spoken sentences.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9 320–324.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Burgoon, J.K., Baesler, E.J. Choosing between micro and macro nonverbal measurement: Application to selected vocalic and kinesic indices. J Nonverbal Behav 15, 57–78 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00997767

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00997767

Keywords

Navigation