Skip to main content
Log in

Trait vs. nontrait conceptualizations of intrinsic/extrinsic motivational orientation

  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study questioned whether intrinsic vs. extrinsic motivation should be conceptualized as a trait among elementary school students. Previous instruments employing question formats that pit an intrinsic orientation against an extrinsic orientation typically result in normal distributions around the midpoint of the scale. Such a pattern could only result from a majority of children endorsing both intrinsic and extrinsic items. In the present study we investigated the hypothesis that such children may be intrinsically motivated in some school subjects but extrinsically motivated in others. Questions designed to assess motivational orientation in each of four academic subjects (math, social studies, English, and science) revealed three types of students: (a) those that were intrinsic in some subjects but extrinsic in others, (b) those that were intrinsic in all school subjects, and (c) those that were extrinsic in all school subjects. It was concluded that for the first group, motivational orientation does not function as a trait, but rather is specific to the academic content. However, for the second and third groups, motivational orientation does appear to be trait-like since children consistently report the same orientation across academic subjects. A similar analysis was applied to consistency vs. change over time where three groups were also identified. Discussion focused on the larger issues involving trait- vs. situation-specific approaches.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport, G. W. (1931). What is a trait of personality?Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 25 368–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1937).Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism.American Psychologist, 33 344–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blyth, D. A., Simmons, R. G., & Carlton-Ford, S. (1983). The adjustment of early adolescents to school transitions.Journal of Early Adolescence, 3 105–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brush, L. (1980).Encouraging girls in mathematics: The problem and the solution. Cambridge, MA: Abt Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Condry, J., & Koslowski, B. (1979). Can education be made “intrinsically interesting” to children? In L. Katz (Ed.),Current topics in early childhood education (Vol. II). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1984). A developmental theory of motivation in the classroom.Teacher Education Quarterly, 11 64–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • deCharms, R. (1968).Personal causation: The internal affective determinants of behavior. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • deCharms, R. (1976).Enhancing motivation in the classroom. New York: Irvington, Halsted-Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1975).Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985).Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S. (1989). Bringing young women to math and science. In. M. Crawford and M. Gentry (Eds.),Gender and thought. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J., & Midgley, C. (1988). Stage-environment fit: Developmentally appropriate classrooms for young adolescents. In R. E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.),Research on motivation in education, Vol. 3: Goals and Cognitions (pp. 139–186). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1990). Changes in academic motivation and self-perceptions during early adolescence. In R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.),Advances in adolescent development: From childhood to adolescence (Vol. 2, pp. 134–155). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles (Parsons), J., Midgley, C., & Adler, T. F. (1984). Grade-related changes in the school environment: Effects on achievement motivation. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.),The development of achievement motivation. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1980). The stability of behavior: II. Implications for psychological research.American Psychologist, 35 790–806.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S., & O'Brien, E. J. (1985). The person-situation debate in historical and current perspective.Psychological Bulletin, 98 513–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Funder, D. C. (1991). Global traits: A neo-Allportian approach to personality.Psychological Science, 2 31–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, E. (1981).Grade, sex, and race differences in academic intrinsic motivation. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Los Angeles.

  • Harter, S. (1981a). A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the classroom: Motivational and informational components.Developmental Psychology, 17 300–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S. (1981b). A model of intrinsic mastery motivation in children: Individual differences and developmental change. In A. Collins (Ed.),Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology, 14 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S. (in press). The relationship between perceived competence, affect, and motivational orientation within the classroom: Process and patterns of change. In A. K. Boggiano & T. Pittman (Eds.),Achievement and motivation: A social-developmental perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press.

  • Harter, S., & Connell, J. P. (1984). A comparison of alternative models of the relationships between academic achievement and children's perceptions of competence, control, and motivational orientation. In J. Nicholls (Ed.),The development of achievement-related cognitions and behaviors. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S., Whitesell, N. R., & Kowalski, P. (in press). Individual differences in the effects of educational transitions of young adolescents' perceptions of competence and motivational orientation.

  • Kanfer, F. H. (1980). Self-management methods. In F. H. Kanfer & A. P. Goldstein (Eds.),Helping people change: A textbook of methods (2nd ed.). New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendrick, D. T., & Funder, D. C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: Lessons from the person-situation debate.American Psychologist, 43 23–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the “overjustification hypothesis.”Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28 129–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1968).Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality.Psychological Review, 80 252–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1979). On the interface of cognition and personality: Beyond the person-situation debate.American Psychologist, 34 740–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1982). Beyond deja vu in the search for cross-situational consistency.Psychological Review, 89 730–755.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J. G. (1979). Quality and equality in intellectual development: The role of motivation in education.American Psychologist, 34 1071–1084.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement.Psychological Monographs, 80(1, Whole No. 609).

  • Simmons, R. G., & Blyth, D. A. (1987).Moving into adolescence: The impact of pubertal change and school context. Hawthron, New York: Aldine de Gruylter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, R. G., Rosenberg, F., & Rosenberg, M. (1973). Distrubance in the self-image at adoleslcence.American Sociological Review, 38 553–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wicklund, R. A. (1978). Three years later. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),Cognitive theories in social psychology. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Harter, S., Jackson, B.K. Trait vs. nontrait conceptualizations of intrinsic/extrinsic motivational orientation. Motiv Emot 16, 209–230 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00991652

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00991652

Keywords

Navigation