Skip to main content
Log in

Naturalistic social cognition: Intersubjectivity in same-sex dyads

  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The research paradigm that Ickes and his colleagues (Ickes, Robertson, Tooke, & Teng, 1986; Ickes & Tooke, 1988) have developed for the study of naturalistic social cognition was used to explore the phenomenon ofintersubjectivity as it naturally occurs in the initial, unstructured interactions of same-sex (male-male and female-female) dyads. The results of two studies revealed evidence of greater similarity (i.e., convergence) in the thought-feeling content of the male dyad members than in that of the female dyad members on measures pertaining to the quality of their reported feelings. These findings were consistent with the assumption that, due to a normatively narrower range of affective involvement in the male-male dyads, the male dyad members were more likely than the female dyad members to monitor closely the emotional tone of their interaction. In direct support of this assumption, there was significantly less nonverbal involvement (i.e., gesturing, smiling, gazing, mutual gaze) in the male-male dyads in both studies, along with corresponding differences in the variability of these behaviors. In addition, the results of both studies revealed serendipitous evidence of greater similarity in the thought-feeling content of the female, than of the male, dyad members on measures pertaining to the degree of metaperspective taking and to thoughts and feelings about other people. The discussion emphasized the need for theorists and researchers in cognitive social psychology to address those genuinelysocial-cognitive (i.e., intersubjective) phenomena that have traditionally been ignored.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abelson, R.P. (1976). Script processing in attitude formation and decision making. In J.S. Carroll & J.W. Payne (Eds.),Cognition and social behavior. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abelson, R.P. (1981). The psychological status of the script concept.American Psychologist, 36 715–729.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altman, I., & Taylor, D.H. (1973).Social Penetration: The Development of Interpersonal Relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antill, J.K. (1983). Sex role complementarity versus similarity in married couples.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45 145–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S.E. (1952).Social Psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S.L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45 196–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brock, T.C. (1967). Communication discrepancy and intent to persuade as determinants of counterargument production.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3 269–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J.T., & Petty, R.E. (1981). Social psychological procedures for cognitive response assessment: The thought-listing technique. In T. Merluzzi, C. Glass, & M. Genest (Eds.),Cognitive Assessment (pp. 309–342). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L.J. (1955). Processes affecting scores on understanding of others and assuming “similarity.”Psychological Bulletin, 52 177–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fincham, F., & O'Leary, K.D. (1983). Causal inferences for spouse behavior in maritally distressed and nondistressed couples.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 1 42–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S.T., & Taylor, S.E. (1984).Social Cognition. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1969).Strategic Interaction. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J.M. (1979).Marital Interaction: Experimental Investigations. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A.G. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response to persuasion, and attitude change. In A.G. Greenwald, T.C. Brock, & T.M. Ostrom (Eds.),Psychological Foundations of Attitudes (pp. 147–170). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958).The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W. (1981). Sex-role influences in dyadic interaction: A theoretical model. In C. Mayo & N. Henley (Eds.),Gender and Nonverbal Behavior (pp. 95–128). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W. (1982). A basic paradigm for the study of personality, roles, and social behavior. In W. Ickes & E.S. Knowles (Eds.),Personality, Roles, and Social Behavior (pp. 305–341). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W. (1983). A basic paradigm for the study of unstructured dyadic interaction. In H. Reis (Ed.),New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science (pp. 5–21). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W. (1985). Sex-role influences on compatibility in relationships. In W. Ickes (Ed.),Compatible and Incompatible Relationships (pp. 187–208). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., & Barnes, R.D. (1977). The role of sex and self-monitoring in unstructured dyadic interactions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 315–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Robertson, E., Tooke, W., & Teng, G. (1986). Naturalistic social cognition: Methodology, assessment, and validation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 66–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Schermer, B., & Steeno, J. (1979). Sex and sex-role influences in same-sex dyads.Social Psychology Quarterly, 42 373–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., & Tooke, W. (1988). The observational method: Studying the interaction of minds and bodies. In S. Duck (Ed.),The Handbook of Personal Relationships: Theory, Research, and Interventions. Chichester: Wiley, 79–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., & Turner, M. (1983). On the social advantages of having an older, opposite-sex sibling: Birth-order influences in mixed-sex dyads.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45 210–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D.A. (1988). The analysis of data from two-person relationships. In S. Duck (Ed.),The Handbook of Personal Relationships: Theory, Research, and Interventions. Chichester: Wiley, 57–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laing, R.D., Phillipson, H., & Lee, A.R. (1966).Interpersonal Perception: A Theory and Method of Research. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamke, L.K., & Bell, N.J. (1982). Sex-role orientation and relationship development in same-sex dyads.Journal of Research in Personality, 16 343–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E.J. (1978). Rethinking the role of thought in social interaction. In J. Harvey, W. Ickes, & R. Kidd (Eds.),New Directions in Attribution Research (Vol. 2, pp. 35–58). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Zajonc, R.B. (1985). The cognitive perspective in social psychology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.),The Handbook of Social Psychology: Third Edition (pp. 137–230). New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G.H. (1934).Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945).Phenomenologie de la Perception. Paris: Gallimard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartre, J.P. (1943).L'Etre et le Neant. Paris: Gallimard. (Transl. H. Barnes,Being and Nothingness. New York: Washington Square Press, 1966.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheler, M. (1923).Wesen und Formen der Sympathie (2nd ed.). Bonn: Friedrich Cohen. (Transl. P. Heath,The Nature of Sympathy. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1954).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R.C., & Abelson, R.P. (1977).Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding: An Inquiry Into Human Knowledge Structures. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1942). Scheler's theory of intersubjectivity and the general thesis of alter ego.Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 2 323–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1948). Sartre's theory of the alter ego.Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 9 181–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1970).On Phenomenology and Social Relations. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaver, P., Pullis, C., & Olds, D. (1980). Report on the LHJ ‘Intimacy Today” Survey. Private research report to the publishers of theLadies' Home Journal.

  • Sillars, A.L. (1981). Attributions and interpersonal conflict resolution. In J.H. Harvey, W. Ickes, & R.F. Kidd (Eds.),New Directions in Attribution Research (Vol. 3, pp. 279–305). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sillars, A.L. (1985). Interpersonal perception in relationships. In W. Ickes (Ed.),Compatible and Incompatible Relationships (pp. 277–305). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swann, W.B., Jr. (1984). Quest for accuracy in person perception: A matter of pragmatics.Psychological Review, 4 457–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooke, W., & Ickes, W. (1988). A measure of adherence to conventional morality.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6 310–334.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The authors would like to express their appreciation to Kristine Arnold, Ray Bristow, Robin Klemick, Laurie Luce, Julie Williams, and Mari Wilson for their assistance in coding the data for the studies reported here.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ickes, W., Tooke, W., Stinson, L. et al. Naturalistic social cognition: Intersubjectivity in same-sex dyads. J Nonverbal Behav 12, 58–82 (1988). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00987352

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00987352

Keywords

Navigation