Skip to main content
Log in

Subitizing: Magical numbers or mere superstition?

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

It is widely believed that humans are endowed with a specialized numerical process, calledsubitizing, which enables them to apprehend rapidly and accurately the numerosity of small sets of objects. A major part of the evidence for this process is a purported discontinuity in the mean response time (RT) versus numerosity curves at about 4 elements, when subjects enumerate up to 7 or more elements in a visual display. In this article, RT data collected in a speeded enumeration experiment are subjected to a variety of statistical analyses, including several tests on the RT distributions. None of these tests reveals a significant discontinuity as numerosity increases. The data do suggest a strong stochastic dominance in RT by display numerosity, indicating that the mental effort required to enumerate does increase with each additional element in the display, both within and beyond the putative subitizing range.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ashby, F. G. (1982). Testing the assumptions of exponential, additive reaction time models.Memory & Cognition, 10, 125–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, F. G., & Townsend, J. T. (1980). Decomposing the reaction time distribution: Pure insertion and selective influence revisited.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 21, 93–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, F. G., Tein, J., & Balakrishnan, J. D. (1991). Response time distributions in memory scanning. Submitted.

  • Balakrishnan, J. D., & Ashby, F. G. (1991). Is subitizing a unique numerical ability?Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 555–564.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1975). The magic number seven after fifteen years. In A. Wilkes & A. Kennedy (Eds.),Studies in long term memory. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burbeck, S. L., & Luce, R. D. (1982). Evidence from auditory simple reaction times for both change and level detectors.Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 117–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, J. M. (1886). Ueber die Trägheit der Netzhaut and des Sehzentrums.Philosophische Studien, 3, 94–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., & Klahr, D. (1975). Span and rate of apprehension in children and adults.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 434–439.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colonius, H. (1990). Possibly dependent probability summation of reaction time.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 34, 253–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hockley, W. E. (1984). Analysis of response time distributions in the study of cognitive processes.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 598–615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, E. L., Lord, M. W., Reese, T. W., & Volkmann, J. (1949). The discrimination of visual number.American Journal of Psychology, 62, 498–525.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., & Wallace, J. G. (1976).Cognitive development: An information processing view. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, L. E. (1984). Perceived numerosity: A comparison of magnitude production, magnitude estimation, and discrimination judgements.Perception & Psychophysics, 35, 536–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laming, D. (1973).Mathematical psychology. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luce, R. D. (1986).Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: An analysis of its component processes.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. R., & Singpurwalla, N. D. (1977).Failure rate estimation using random smoothing. National Technical Information Service, No. AD-A040999/5ST.

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity to process information.Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967).Cognitive psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Oeffelen, M. P., & Vos, P. G. (1982a). Configurational effects on the enumeration of dots: Counting by groups.Memory & Cognition, 10, 396–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Oeffelen, M. P., & Vos, P. G. (1982b). A probabilistic model for the discrimination of visual number.Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 163–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, T., Kikuchi, T., & Ichihara, S. (1981). Span of attention, backward masking, and reaction time.Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 106–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parzen, E. (1960).Modern probability theory and its applications. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, W. W., Birdsall, T. G., & Fox, W. C. (1954). The theory of signal detectability.Trans. IRE Professional Group on Information Theory, PGIT-4, 171–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval.Psychological Review, 85, 59–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1988). A note on mimicking additive reaction time models.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 32, 192–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, B. W. (1986).Density estimation for statistics and data analysis. London: Chapman & Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D., & Langheinrich, D. (1982). What is magic about the magical number four?Psychological Research, 44, 283–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starkey, P., & Cooper, R. G. (1980). Perception of number by human infants.Science, 210, 1033–1035.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Starkey, P., Spelke, E. S., & Gelman, R. (1983). Detection of intermodal numerical correspondences by human infants.Science, 222 179–181.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method. In W. G. Koster (Ed.),Attention and performance (Vol. 2). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T. (1988). The truth and consequences of ordinal differences in statistical distributions: Toward a theory of hierarchical inference.Psychological Bulletin, 108, 551–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Szeliski, V. (1924). Relation between the quantity perceived and the time of perception.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 7, 135–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954).Experimental psychology. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1896). Grundriß der Psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Balakrishnanl, J.D., Ashby, F.G. Subitizing: Magical numbers or mere superstition?. Psychol. Res 54, 80–90 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00937136

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00937136

Keywords

Navigation