Skip to main content
Log in

Continuous selection

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

A model of target selection in continuous search tasks is outlined. The model was designed to account for two basic observations that are difficult to cover within the framework of the traditional target-control-type models of search, namely, pure detection (detection without identification) and pseudotarget detection (detection of new items not to be searched for). The model combines two basic assumptions: first, that targets are detected by default, that is, by virtue of the fact that they do not fit into an internal model of to-be-expected events (target detection by default) and second, that this internal model is generated, maintained, and updated on the basis of the nontarget information encountered during search (integration of non-target information). Furthermore, it is assumed that non-target integration and target detection can both be carried out at several processing levels simultaneously. The evidence available to support and develop the model is reviewed, and some of its general implications for a theory of selective attention are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport, A. (1987). Selection for action: some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brand, J. (1971). Classification without identification in visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 178–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcoran, D. W. J. (1966). An acoustic factor in letter cancellation. Nature, 210, 658.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcoran, D. W. J. & Weening, D. L. (1968). Acoustic factors in visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 83–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1983). Category effects in visual search: a failure to replicate the “oh-zero” phenomenon. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 221–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heijden, A. H. C. van der (1987). Selective attention in vision. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. (1973). Effects of letter-names on visual search. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 90–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingling, N. W. (1974). Categorization: a mechanism for rapid information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 239–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., Gleitman, H. (1972). A conceptual category effect in visual search: O as a letter or as digit. Perception & Psychophysics, 12, 457–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehrer, L. (1985). Über Klassifikationsleistungen beim visuellen Suchen. Untersuchungen zur Funktionsgrundlage des Kategorieneffektes. Dissertation, University of Bielefeld.

  • Krueger, L. E. (1984). The category effect in visual search depends on physical rather than conceptual differences. Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 558–564.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer-Eppler, W. (1969). Grundlagen und Anwendungen der Informationstheorie. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B. (1980). Short-term recognition memory. In R. S. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and Performance, Vol. VIII. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nattkemper, D., & Prinz, W. (1984). Costs and benefits of redundancy in visual search. In A. Gale & F. Johnson (Eds.), Theoretical and applied aspects of eye-movement research. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1963). Decision-time without reaction-time: Experiments in visual scanning. American Journal of Psychology, 76, 376–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U., Novick, R., & Lazar, R. (1963). Searching for ten targets simultaneously. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 17, 955–961.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olst, G. H. van (1971) The orienting reflex. The Hague, Paris: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1977). Memory control of visual search. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and performance, Vol. VI. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1978 a). Was lernt man aus dem Umgang mit A, B, C, D... für den Umgang mit a, b, c, d ...? Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 25, 230–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1978 b). Auditive und artikulatorische Störbarkeit visueller Suchprozesse. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 25, 441–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1979). Integration of information in visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 287–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1983 a). Wahrnehmung und Tätigkeitssteuerung. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1983 b). Zeitliche Integration von Information bei kontinuierlichem Suchen. Psychologische Beiträge, 25, 57–70

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1983 c). Asymmetrical control areas in continuous visual search. In R. Groner, C. Menz, D. F. Fisher, & R. A. Monty (Eds.), Eye movements and psychological functions: International views. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W., Hartlich, S., & Lahmeyer, W. (1972). Nicht-visueller Transfer in einer visuellen Suchaufgabe. Psychologische Forschung, 35, 218–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W., & Kehrer, L. (1982). Recording detection distances in continuous visual search. In R. Groner, & P. Fraisse (Eds.), Cognition and eye movements. Berlin: Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W., Meinecke, C., & Hielscher, M. (in press). Effects of stimulus degradation on category search. Acta Psychologica.

  • Prinz, W., & Nattkemper, D. (1986). Effects of secondary tasks on search performance. Psychological Research, 48, 47–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W., & Nattkemper, D. (1987). Integrating non-target information in continuous search. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Prinz, W., & Prinz, U. (1985). Verarbeitung redundanter Zeichensequenzen durch Kinder im Grundschulalter. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 27, 210–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W., Tweer, R., & Feige, R. (1974). Context control of search behavior: Evidence from a ‘hurdling’ technique. Acta Psychologica, 38, 73–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrbaugh, J. W. (1984). The orienting reflex: Performance and central nervous system manifestations. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.). Varieties of attention. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. London: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This paper is a selective summary of experimental work done in collaboration with Dieter Nattkemper and Cristina Meinecke and supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (grant Pr 118/5)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Prinz, W. Continuous selection. Psychol. Res 48, 231–238 (1986). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309087

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309087

Keywords

Navigation