Skip to main content
Log in

Inflection, frequency, and the word superiority effect

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

It has been known for nearly 100 years that single words presented tachistoscopically are better recognized than arbitrary letter strings. The present experiments investigate the impact of the morphological structure of words and of word frequency on the word superiority effect. It has been shown that inflected word forms show higher reaction time and error scores in the lexical decision task and higher error scores in the Reicher-Wheeler paradigm. Word frequency also determines lexical decision time. However, no frequency effect in the Reicher-Wheeler paradigm could be found. The results are discussed in the framework of McClelland and Rumelhart's (1981) model of context effects in letter search.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baron, J. (1978). The word superiority effect: Perceptual learning from reading. In: Estes W. (ed), Handbook of Learning and Cognitive Processes (pp 131–167). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J., & Thurston, I. (1973). An analysis of the word superiority effect. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 207–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, J.M. (1886). The time taken up by cerebral operations. Mind 11, 220–242; 377–392; 524–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect-fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 257–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, E.B. (1978). Generalization effects vs. random effects: Is sZ tl a source of type 1 or type 2 error? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 243–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foote, W., & Havens, L.C. (1965). Stimulus frequency: Determinant of perception or response? Psychonomic Science, 2, 153–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. & Guinet, L. (1971). Perception of inflections in brief visual presentations of words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 182–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E., Pick, A., Osser, H., & Hammond, M. (1962). The role of grapheme-phoneme-correspondence in the perception of words. American Journal of Psychology, 75, 554–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gräbnitz, V. (1982). Häufigkeitsuntersuchungen zum ‚Korpus der deutschen Gegenwartssprache’. Master's thesis. Institut f. Phonetik & Sprachliche Kommunikation, University of Munich.

  • Günther, H. (1983a). Studien zur visuellen Worterkennung. Forschungsberichte des Instituts für Phonetik & Sprachliche Kommunikation der Universität München (FIPKM), 18, 1–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Günther, H. (1983b). Zur methodischen und theoretischen Notwendigkeit zweifacher statistischer Analyse sprachpsychologischer Experimente. Sprache & Kognition, 2, 279–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. (1980). Is there a lexicality component in the word superiority effect? Perception & Psychophysics, 28, 179–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. (1982). Orthography and Word recognition in Reading. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J.C. (1978). A test of the sophisticated guessing theory. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 123–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hockett, C. (1954). Two models of grammatical description. Word 10, 210–231. Again in: M. Joos (ed.): Readings in Linguistics I (1966), 386–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaeding. F.W. (1898). Häufigkeitswörterbuch der deutschen Sprache. Berlin.

  • Lukatela, G., Gligorijević, B., Kostić, A., & Turvey, M.T. (1980) Representation of inflected nouns in the internal lexicon. Memory & Cognition, 8, 415–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manelis, L. (1974). The effect of meaningfulness in tachistoscopic word perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 182–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manelis, L. (1977). The effect of frequency and meaningfulness in tachistoscopic word perception. American Journal of Psychology, 90, 296–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D.W., Taylor, G.A. Venezky, R.L., Jastrzembski, J.E., & Lucas, P.A. (1980). Letter and Word Perception. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, P.H. (1974). Morphology. An Introduction to the Theory of Word Structure. Cambridge: UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., & Johnston, J.C. (1977). The role of familiar units in the perception of words and nonwords. Perception & Psychophysics, 22, 249–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., & Rumelhart, D.E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception. Part I: An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A., Bruner, J.S., & Postman, L. (1954). Familiarity of letter sequence and tachistoscopic identification. Journal of General Psychology, 50, 129–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murell, G. & Morton, J. (1974). Word recognition and morphemic structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 963–968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortmann, W.D. (1975). Hochfrequente deutsche Wortformen I. München: Goethe Institut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reicher, G.M. (1969). Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 274–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichling, A. (1935). Het woord. Nijmegen.

  • Rumelhart, D.E., & McClelland, J.L. (1982). An interactive model of context effects in letter perception: Part II. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review, 89, 60–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spoehr, K. (1978). Phonological encoding in visual word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 127–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spoehr, K., & Smith, E.E. (1975). The role of orthographic and phonotactic rules in perceiving letter patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 1, 21–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M. (1981). Prefix stripping revisited. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 289–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Forster, K.I. (1976). Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 607–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whaley, C.P. (1978). Word-nonword-classification time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 143–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, D.D. (1970). Processes in word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 59–85.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This paper is mainly (Experiments I and III) based on the first author's ‘Habilitationsschrift’ from October, 1982 (cf. Günther 1983a).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Günther, H., Gfroerer, S. & Weiss, L. Inflection, frequency, and the word superiority effect. Psychol. Res 46, 261–281 (1984). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308888

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308888

Keywords

Navigation