Skip to main content
Log in

Structures and strategies: Where are we now?

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

Psychological opinion tends to sway between a view that people are almost infinitely flexible and a view that there are definite and unchangeable fundamental mechanisms that always work in the same way and set limits of flexibility. A popular compromise has been to suppose that ‘lower’ mechanisms are as determinate as typewriters, while ‘higher’ ones are unanalysable, autonomous, and perhaps even beyond scientific enquiry. The point of view in this paper favours a different compromise; very early and elementary operations are thought to be done in different ways, depending on the goals and experience of the person. There is, however, a good deal of constraint, which allows laws and principles to be laid down. Yet, there is also a degree of strategic flexibility in the operations actually performed that goes further than would be allowed by theorists of a different persuasion. The selective operations of attention are not due to unalterable mechanisms, nor does there seem to be convincing evidence of universal and invariant computation of all aspects of the input. What is computed, briefly, is the minimum necessary to perform the task before the person.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, R., & Berry, C. (1981). Memory for sentences: The subject superiority effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 295–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. II, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1983). Working memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B 302, 311–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. VIII, pp. 47–90). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., Lewis, V., Eldridge, M., & Thomson, N. (1984). Attention and retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 518–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and stress. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1981). From the percept to the cognitive structure. In A. D. Baddeley, & J. Long (Eds.). Attention & Performance (Vol. IX, pp. 1–24). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1982). Task combination and selective intake of information. Acta Psychologica, 50, 253–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1984). The Maltese cross: A new simplistic model for memory. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 55–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1981). Articulatory suppression and the grouping of successive stimuli. Psychological Research, 43, 57–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M. (1983). On the transfer of information from temporary to permanent memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B302, 341–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1980). The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 272–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon identification of target in a non-search task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • FitzGerald, J. P., Broadbent D. E. (1985). Order of report and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 11, 217–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S., & Broadbent, D. E. (1984). Combining attributes in specified and categorized target search: Further evidence for strategic differences. Memory & Cognition, 12, 329–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, R. L., & Harris, J. P. (1975). Illusion-destruction by appropriate scaling. Perception, 4, 203–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J. E., Nelson, B., & Houck, M. R. (1983). The role of attentional resources in automatic detection. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 379–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G. V. (1983). Structure of the recall process. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B302, 373–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman, & D. R. Davies, (Eds.), Varieties of Attention (pp. 29–61). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, A. L., & Cooper, M. B. (1985). Man-machine interface issues in the construction and use of an expert system. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 22, 91–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLean, J. P., Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1982). Combining attributes in rapid sequential visual presentation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 171–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, C. (1984). Pronoun identification: The coordination of available information. Thesis approved for Ph. O. degree, University of Oxford.

  • Morton, J. (1981). The status of information processing models of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B295, 387–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Routh, D. (1976). An ‘across-the-board’ modality effect in immediate serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 285–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Fisk, A. D. (1984). Automatic category search and its transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 10, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M. (in press). Attention. In R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, G. Lindzey, & R. D. Luce, (Eds.), Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology, 2nd edition. New York: Wiley.

  • Shiffrin, R. M., Dumais, S. T., & Schneider, W. (1981). Characteristics of automatism. In J. Long & A. D. Baddeley, (Eds.), Attention & Performance (Vol. IX, pp. 223–238). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1983). Reason in human affairs. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs, 74 (Whole Number 498), 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, P., & Williams, F. (1968). Prompted word replacement in active and passive sentences. Language and Speech, 11, 220–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilhite, S. C. (1982). Sentence coding: Tests of the address-contents model and the fragmentation-conceptual hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34A, 259–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, P. (1969). Transformation and the understanding of sentences. Language and Speech, 12, 156–160.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Broadbent, D. Structures and strategies: Where are we now?. Psychol. Res 49, 73–79 (1987). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308671

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308671

Keywords

Navigation