Skip to main content
Log in

Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements

2. The effects of changing object size

  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

  1. 1.

    Subjects were instructed to reach and grasp cylindrical objects, using a precision grip. The objects were two concentric dowels made of translucent material placed at 35 cm from the subject. The inner (“small”) dowel was 10 cm high and 1.5 cm in diameter. The outer (“large”) dowel was 6 cm high and 6 cm in diameter. Prehension movements were monitored using a Selspot system. The displacement of a marker placed at the wrist level was used as an index for the transport of the hand at the location of the object. Markers placed at the tips of the thumb and the index finger were used for measuring the size of aperture of the finger grip.

  2. 2.

    Kinematics of transport and grasp components were computed from the filtered displacement signals. Movement time (MT), time to peak velocity (TPV) and time to peak deceleration (TPD) of the wrist, time to peak velocity of grip aperture (TGV), time to maximum grip aperture (TGA) were the main parameters used for comparing the movements in different conditions. Spatial paths of the wrist, thumb and index markers were reconstructed in two dimensions. Variability of the spatial paths over repeated trials was computed as the surface of the ellipses defined by X and Y standard deviations from the mean path.

  3. 3.

    Computer controlled illumination of one of the dowels was the signal for reaching toward that dowel. Blocks of trials were made to the small dowel and to the large dowel. Mean MT during blocked trials was 550 ms. The acceleration phase of the movements (measured by parameter TPV) represented 33% of MT. About half of MT (52%) was spent after TPD in a low velocity phase while the hand was approaching the object. This kinematic pattern was not influenced by whether movements were directed at small or large dowels.

  4. 4.

    Grip aperture progressively increased during transport of the hand. TGA corresponded to about 60% of MT, that is, maximum grip aperture was reached during the low velocity phase of transport. Following TGA, fingers closed around the object until contact was made. This pattern of grip formation differed whether the movement was directed at the large or the small dowel: TGA occurred often earlier for the small dowel, and the size of the maximum grip aperture was larger for the large dowel. Variability of both the wrist and finger spatial paths was larger during the first half of MT, and tended to become very low as the hand approached the dowels.

  5. 5.

    Selective perturbations of dowel size were randomly produced at the onset of prehension movements. Perturbations involved increase in object size (the illumination was suddenly shifted from the small to the large dowel, S-L perturbation), or decrease in object size (from the large to the small dowel, L-S perturbations). During S-L perturbations MT was increased by 175 ms on average. As TPV and TPD did not differ from control unperturbed movements, increase in MT was entirely due to lengthening of the low velocity phase following TPD.

  6. 6.

    Grip formation was affected by the perturbation. Grip aperture first peaked to the size corresponding to the small dowel, then reincreased for accomodating the size of the large dowel. The time where grip aperture reincreased (as measured on the curve of grip velocity) occurred 329.8 ms on average after movement onset. Variability of wrist and finger spatial paths was increased with respect to controls, but it remained low during the final phase of the movement. L-S perturbations had similar effects, though attenuated with respect to S-L perturbations.

  7. 7.

    This relatively long time taken to initiate corrections in response to object size perturbations contrasts with the short time (about 100 ms) for initiating corrections during perturbations of object position. This difference suggests some degree of independence of the mechanisms generating finger movements during grip formation from those generating transport of the hand. In addition, the kinematic coupling of the two components (demonstrated here by lengthening of the low velocity phase of the transport during correction of finger grip size) suggests the existence of a different mechanism subserving temporal coordination of the two components.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbs JH, Gracco VL (1984) Control of complex motor gestures: orofacial muscle responses to load perturbations of lip during speech. J Neurophysiol 51: 705–723

    Google Scholar 

  • Alstermark B, Gorska T, Lundberg A, Petterson LG (1990) Integration in descending motor pathways controlling the forelimb in the cat. 16. Visually guided switching of target-reaching. Exp Brain Res 80: 1–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib MA (1985) Schemas for the temporal organization of behavior. Hum Neurobiol 4: 63–72

    Google Scholar 

  • Bizzi E, Accorneo N, Chapple W, Hogan N (1984) Posture control and trajectory formation during arm movement. J Neurosci 4: 2738–2744

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock D, Grossberg S (1988) Neural dynamics of planned arm movements. Emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation. Psychol Rev 95: 49–90

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole KJ, Abbs JH (1987) Kinematic and electromyographic responses to perturbation of a rapid grasp. J Neurophysiol 57: 1498–1510

    Google Scholar 

  • Faugier-Grimaud S, Frenois C, Stein DG (1978) Effects of posterior parietal lesions on visually guided behavior in monkeys. Neuropsychologia 16: 151–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Franks IM, Sanderson DJ, van Donkelaar P (1990) A comparison of directly recorded and derived acceleration data in movement control research. Hum Mov Sci 9: 573–582

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentilucci M, Castiello U, Corradini ML, Scarpa M, Umilta C, Rizzolatti G (1991) Influence of different types of grasping on the transport component of prehension movements. Neuropsychologia 29: 361–378

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos AP, Kalaska JF, Massey JT (1981) Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty and change in target location. J Neurophysiol 46: 725–743

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodale MA, Pélisson D, Prablanc C (1986) Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature 320: 748–750

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard P, Wing AM (1991) Remote responses to perturbation in human prehension. Neurosci Lett (in press)

  • Hofsten C Von, Rönnqvist L (1988) Preparation for grasping an object: a developmental study. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept] 14: 610–621

    Google Scholar 

  • Iberall T, Bingham G, Arbib MA (1986) Opposition space as a structuring concept for the analysis of skilled hand movements. In: Heuer H, Fromm C (eds) Generation and modulation of action pattern (Exp Brain Res Series 15). Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 158–173

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M, Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects. In: Long J, Baddeley A (eds) Attention and Performance IX. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 153–168

  • Jeannerod M (1984) The timing of natural prehension movements. J Mot Behav 16: 235–254

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1986) The formation of finger grip during prehension. A cortically mediated visuomotor pattern. Behav Brain Res 19: 99–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1990) The interaction of visual and proprioceptive cues in controlling reaching movements. In: Humphrey DR, Freund HJ (eds) Motor Control: concepts and issues. Wiley, New York, pp 277–291

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M, Michel F, Prablanc C (1984) The control of hand movements in a case of hemianaesthesia following a parietal lesion. Brain 107: 899–920

    Google Scholar 

  • Mac Neilage PF (1970) Motor control of serial ordering of speech. Psychol Rev 77: 182–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Marteniuk RG, Leavitt JL, MacKenzie CL, Athenes S (1990) Functional relationships between grasp and transport components in a prehension task. Hum Mov Sci 9: 149–176

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulignan Y, MacKenzie CL, Marteniuk R, Jeannerod M (1990) The coupling of arm and finger movements during prehension. Exp Brain Res 79: 431–436

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulignan Y, MacKenzie CL, Marteniuk R, Jeannerod M (1991) Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements. I. The effects of changing object position. Exp Brain Res 83: 502–512

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pélisson D, Prablanc C, Goodale MA, Jeannerod M (1986) Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb. II. Evidence of fast unconscious processes correcting the trajectory of the hand to the final position of a double-step stimulus. Exp Brain Res 62: 303–311

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perenin MT, Vighetto A (1988) Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. Brain 111: 643–674

    Google Scholar 

  • Prablanc C, Echallier JF, Jeannerod M, Komilis E (1979) Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target. II. Static and dynamic visual cues in the control of hand movement. Biol Cybern 35: 183–187

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Lacquaniti F (1983) Modification of trajectory of a pointing movement in response to a change in target location. J Neurophysiol 49: 548–564

    Google Scholar 

  • Taira M, Mine S, Georgopoulos AP, Murata A, Sakata H (1990) Parietal cortex neurons of the monkey related to the visual guidance of hand movements. Exp Brain Res 83: 29–36

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Viviani P, Terzuolo C (1980) Space-time invariance in learned motor skills. In: Stelmach GE, Requin J (eds) Tutorials in motor behavior. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp 525–533

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace SA, Weeks DL (1988) Temporal constraints in the control of prehensive movements. J Mot Behav 20: 81–105

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wing AM, Fraser C (1983) The contribution of the thumb to reaching movements. Q J Exp Psychol 35A: 297–309

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing AM, Turton A, Fraser C (1986) Grasp size and accuracy of approach in reaching. J Mot Behav 18: 245–260

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Supported by INSERM, Paris

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Paulignan, Y., Jeannerod, M., MacKenzie, C. et al. Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements. Exp Brain Res 87, 407–420 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00231858

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00231858

Key words

Navigation