Skip to main content
Log in

Head movement propensity

  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

In studies of human eye-head coordinated saccadic gaze shifts, different laboratories have found greatly different magnitudes of head movements for a given amplitude of gaze shift (head movement gain). The present study was conducted to examine why, and to quantify volitional head movements. Fixation/target lights were located at 20° and 40° on either side of a central light. There were two tasks or modes. In the non-aligned mode, gaze fixation (first light) was followed by a gaze step to the target (second light) accurately and quickly. In the head-aligned mode, the head was aligned within 3° of the first fixation light (i.e., initial starting position) before the step sequence began. In both non-aligned and head-aligned modes, subject instructions pertaining to the second target light concerned only gaze; there was no requisite head position. The head movement propensity of nine subjects was ranked according to the mean gain (head/target amplitude) of two 40° jumps (0–40° and — 20 to + 20°) in the non-aligned mode. This ranking method clearly identified extreme head-movers and non-movers. The moderate movers were further characterized by three additional criteria, derived by comparing the gains in different jumps, which varied in starting position and amplitude. First, when the two 40° jumps were compared, typically the gain of non-movers was less in the symmetric jump (- 20 to + 20°) with the gain of the head-movers was greater in the symmetric jump. Second, in the head-aligned mode the gain of non-movers progressively increased when the starting position was progressively moved eccentrically, whereas the gain of head-movers increased only slightly, if at all. Third, when the gains of two symmetric (40° and 80°) jumps were compared, the head-movers consistently had opposite trends from non-movers. These three comparative criteria and the initial criterion together define head movement propensity. To explain the above observations, three effects are proposed. First, a “midline-attraction” effect causes resistance to movement away from the midline in non-movers and an increase in movement amplitude if the jump starts eccentrically. Second, a “resetting” effect occurs when the eccentricity of the jump is varied; the stopping position is reset closer to the target. Third, an “awareness/arousal” effect increases the gain in the head-aligned mode due to the intrinsic nature of the alignment procedure. Minor physical movement impediments were added to the recording apparatus; these impediments essentially had no effect on head movement gain, and only slightly altered peak head velocity of large movements. Thus, nonphysical variations in eliciting gaze shifts likely determine gain in different laboratories; gain is reliable if measured on a relative scale among similarly tested subjects. Individuals have an innate behavioral propensity to move their head; this may in part reflect the individual's method of constructing stable coordinate systems relating internal to external reference frames, with head-movers initially choosing extrinsic spatial or earth-fixed coordinates and non-movers choosing intrinsic coordinates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Afanador AJ, Aitsebaomo AP (1982) The range of eye movements through progressive mulitfocals. Optomet Monogr 73: 82–88

    Google Scholar 

  • Bahill AT, Clark MR, Stark L (1975) Glissades — eye movements generated by mismatched components of the saccadic motoneuronal control signal. Math Biosci 26: 303–318

    Google Scholar 

  • Bard C, Fleury M, Paillard J (1992) Head orienting and aiming accuracy. In: Berthoz A, Graff W, Vidal P-P (eds) Head-neck sensory-motor system. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 582–586

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes GR (1979) Vestibulo-ocular function during co-ordinated head and eye movements to acquire visual targets. J Physiol (Lond) 287: 127–147

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker W (1989) Metrics. In: Wurtz R, Goldberg M (eds) The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements. Elsevier, Amsterdam New York, pp 13–67

    Google Scholar 

  • Bizzi E, Kalil RE, Morasso P (1972) Central programming and peripheral feedback during eye-head coordination in monkeys. Bibl Ophthal 82: 220–232

    Google Scholar 

  • Delreux V, Abeele SV, Lefevre P, Roucoux A (1991) Eye-head co-ordination: influence of eye position on the control of head movement amplitude. In: Paillard J (eds) Brain and space. Oxford University Press, Oxford pp 38–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Droulez J, Berthoz A (1991) A neural network model of sensoritopic maps with predictive short-term memory properties. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 88: 9653–9657

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller JH (1981) Eye and head movements during vestibular stimulation in the alert rabbit. Brain Res 205: 363–381

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller JH (1992a) Comparison of head movement strategies among mammals. In: Berthoz A, Graff W, Vidal P-P (eds) Head-neck sensory-motor system. Oxford University Press, Oxford pp 101–112

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller JH (1992b) Single vestibular unit recording in alert cat during active and passive head movements. In: Berthoz A, Graff W, Vidal P-P (eds) Head-neck sensory-motor system. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 279–288

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg ME, Wurtz RH (1972) Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. I. Visual receptive fields of single neurons. J Neurophysiol 35: 542–559

    Google Scholar 

  • Gresty MA (1974) Coordination of head and eye movements to fixate continuous and intermittent targets. Vision Res 14: 395–403

    Google Scholar 

  • Guitton D, Volle M (1987) Gaze control in humans: eye head coordination during orienting movements to targets within and beyond the oculomotor range. J Neurophysiol 58: 427–459

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jampel RS, Shi DX (1992) The primary position of the eyes, the resetting Saccade, and the transverse visual head plane. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 33: 2501–2510

    Google Scholar 

  • Pozzo T, Berthoz A, Lefort L (1992) Head Kinematics during complex movements. In: Head-neck sensory-motor system. Berthoz A, Graff W, Vidal P-P (eds) Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 587–590

    Google Scholar 

  • Seagraves M, Goldberg M (1992) Properties of eye and head movements evoked by electrical stimulation of the monkey superior colliculus. In: Head-neck sensory-motor system. Berthoz A, Graff W, Vidal P-P (eds) Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 292–295

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson D, Bahara PS (1986) Combined eye-head gaze shifts in the primate. I. Metrics. J Neurophysiol 56: 1542–1557

    Google Scholar 

  • Viirre E, Tweed D, Milner K, Vilis T (1986) A reexamination of the gain of the vestibuloocular reflex. J Neurophysiol 56: 439–450

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zangemeister WH, Stark L (1981) Active head rotations and eyehead coordination. Ann NY Acad Sci 374: 540–559

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fuller, J.H. Head movement propensity. Exp Brain Res 92, 152–164 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00230391

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00230391

Key words

Navigation