Skip to main content

The Blind and the Paralytic: Supporting argumentation in Everyday and Scientific Issues

  • Chapter
Arguing to Learn

Part of the book series: Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning ((CULS,volume 1))

Abstract

Traditionally, we define a literate person as somebody who knows how to read, write and calculate. In other words, the literate person is able to handle the signs and symbols used by the society in which she lives. However, this definition is fuzzy. What do we mean by knowing how to read? Do we mean knowing how to decipher the meaning of canonical texts that teachers have themselves constructed or accepted from people who have been invested the authority to give the exact meaning of texts (writers of teaching materials for example)? Indeed, teachers’ written guides often give pedagogical advice how to analyze texts, to extract the main idea: the role of the reader is then to reconstruct a meaning (see Kintsch’s situation model, 1986). Also, when students analyze a text, their objective is generally not more than to comply with the demands of a curriculum, that is (in general) to master skills. However, while literacy definitely involves analytical skills, confining it to a series of skills is far too restricted, and puts aside contextual factors in literacy. As noticed by Olson (1994), in opposition to children’s oral propensity to identify speech acts in conversation, when interpreting written signs they have difficulties to identify their illocutionary force. In other words, children may have difficulties in reading partly because they don’t know for which purpose the texts with which they are presented with were written. The approach to literacy we adopt here conveys the versatility that any literate human must exhibit, to be able to communicate actions through different modes and to negotiate them. This practically means that the literate human should express actions in various modes such as reading or writing, with flexible motives such as clarifying an issue, presenting, demonstrating, defending, or convincing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Andriessen, J. and Coirier P. (1999). Foundations of Argumentative Text Processing (pp. 179–202). Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azmitia, M. (1996). Peer interactive minds: Developmental, theoretical, and methodological issues. In Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., and et al. (Eds.), Interactive minds: Life-span perspectives on the social foundation of cognition (pp. 133–162 ). New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antaki, C. (1994). Explaining and Arguing. The social Organization of Accounts. Sage Publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M. (1999). Argumentation and constructive interaction. In J. Andriessen, P. Coirier (Eds.), Foundations of Argumentative Text Processing (pp. 179–202). Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M. (2002). Computer-Mediated Argumentative Interactions for the co-elaboration of scientific notions. This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cazden, C. B. (1988). Classroom discourse. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann Educational Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen C., and Elstein, S. (1991). Informal Reasoning in the Medical Profession. In J. F. Voss, D. N. Perkins, and J. W. Segal (Eds.), Informal Reasoning and Education (pp. 17–35 ). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coirier, P., Andriessen, J. and Chanquoy, L. (1999). From Planning to Translating the Specificity of Argumentative Writing. In J. Andriessen, P. Coirier (Eds.), Foundations of Argumentative Text Processing (pp. 1–28). Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davenport, P. and Howe, C. (1999). Conceptual gain and successful problem-solving in primary school mathematics. Educational Studies, 25, 55–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davenport, P., Howe, C., and Noble, A. (2000). Peer interaction and the coordination of knowledge. Swiss Journal of Educational Sciences, 22(3), 481–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, K. (1993). Concept Discovery in a Scientific Domain. Cognitive Science,/7(3), 397–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duschl, R. A. (1990). Restructuring Science education: The importance of theories and their development. New York, Columbia University: Teachers College Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Duschl, R. A., and Gitomer, D.H. (1991). Epistemological perspectives on conceptual change: Implications for educational practice. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28, 839–858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duschl, R. A., and Hamilton, R. (1992). Philosophy of Science, Cognitive Psychology, and Educational Theory and Practice. Suny Series in Science Education. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hershkowitz, R., and Schwarz, B. B. (1999). Reflective processes in a technology-based mathematics classroom. Cognition and Instruction, 17, 66–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kedem, O. (1999). “Temporarily Definitive”. The planning, development, production and educational implementation of a series of films and its effects on Students’ conceptions and views regarding the Nature of Science. Doctoral Dissertation. University of Salford, Salford, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1986). Learning From Text. In L.B. Resnick (Ed.). Knowing, Learning, and Instruction pp(25–46). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Kuhn, D. (1991). The skills of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D (1993). Science as argument: Implication for teaching and learning scientific thinking skills. Scientific Education, 77, 319–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Ross, L., and Lepper, M. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effect of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2098–2109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Means, M. L. and Voss, J. F. (1996). Who reasons well? Two studies of informal reasoning among children of different grade, ability and knowledge levels. Cognition and Instruction, 14(2), 139–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meichtry, Y..J. (1993). The Impact of Science Curricula on Students Views About the Nature of Science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 30(4), 429–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, K. and Woodruff, E. (1997). Consensually driven explanation in science teaching. Science Education, 81, 173–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson. S. (1992). The cognitive skill of theory articulation: A neglected aspect of science education. Science and Education, 1, 181–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. (1996). The World on paper. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orsolini, M., (1993). “Dwarfs do not shoot”: An Analysis of Children’s Justifications. Cognition and Instruction, 11(3 and 4), 281–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orsolini, M., and Pontecorvo, C. (1992). Children’s talk in classroom discussion. Cognition and Instruction, 9, 113–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D. N., Farady, M., and Bushey, B. (1991). Everyday reasoning and the roots of intelligence. In J. F. Voss, D. N. Perkins, and J. W. Segal (Eds.), Informal Reasoning and Education (pp. 83–105 ). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomeroy, D. (1993). Implications of teachers’ beliefs about the nature of science: Comparisons of the beliefs of scientists, secondary science teachers, and elementary teachers. Science Education, 77, 261–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pontecorvo, C. and Girardet, H., (1993). Arguing and reasoning in understanding historical topics. Cognition and instruction, 11(3 and 4), 365–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. B., Salmon, M., Zeitz, C. M., Wathen, S. H., and Holowchak, M. (1993). Reasoning in conversation. Cognition and instruction, 11(3 and 4), 347–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., and Chavajay, P. (1995). What’s become of research on the cultural basis of cognitive development? American Psychologist, 50 (10) 859–877.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roschelle, J. (1992). Learning by collaboration: convergent conceptual change. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3, 235–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 31–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. B., Benaya, L. and Shemaya, H. (2001). Symmetry and Learning in Argumentation Among Peers. Paper presented at the First International Conference on Communication, Problem-Solving and Learning, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. B., and Hershkowitz, R. (1995). Argumentation and reasoning in a technology-based class. Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 731–735, Pittsburgh, PA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. B., Neuman, Y. and Biezuner, S. (2000). Two “wrongs” may make a right…If they argue together! Cognition andInstruction, 18(4), 461–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. B., Neuman, Y. and Gil, J., and Ilya, M. (2002). Construction of collective and individual knowledge in argumentative activity: An empirical study. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12(2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Songer N., and Linn, M (1991). How Do Student’s Views of Science Influence Knowledge Integration? Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28 (9), 761–796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, U. M., and Joos, M. (2000). Interactive Minds — A paradigm for the study of the social-interactive nature of human cognition and its life-span development. Swiss Journal of Educational Sciences, 22(3), 559–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, N., and Miller, C.A. (1991). twin.., you lose: the development of argumentative thinking. In J. F. Voss, D.N. Perkins and J. Segal (Eds.), Informal reasoning and Instruction (pp. 265–290 ). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, N., and Miller, C.A. (1993). The development of memory and reasoning skill in argumentative contexts: evaluating, explaining, and generating evidence. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology~Vol. 4 (pp. 235–285). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Suthers. D. (2002). This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toulmin, S.E. (1958). The uses of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tweney (1991). In J. Voss, D Perkins, and J. Segal (Eds.), Informal Reasoning and Education. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Bruggen, J. M. and Kirschner, P. A. (2002). Designing external representations to support solving wicked problems. This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voss, J. F. (1991). Informal Reasoning and International Relations.. In J. F. Voss, D. N. Perkins, and J. W. Segal (Eds.), Informal Reasoning and Education (pp. 37–58 ). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. and Schooler, J. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodruff, E. and Meyer, K. (1997). Explanations from intra-and inter-group discourse: students building knowledge in the science classroom. Research in Science Education, 27 (1), 25–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2003 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Schwarz, B.B., Glassner, A. (2003). The Blind and the Paralytic: Supporting argumentation in Everyday and Scientific Issues. In: Andriessen, J., Baker, M., Suthers, D. (eds) Arguing to Learn. Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, vol 1. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0781-7_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0781-7_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-6320-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-017-0781-7

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics