Skip to main content

Representational Guidance for Collaborative Inquiry

  • Chapter
Arguing to Learn

Part of the book series: Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning ((CULS,volume 1))

Abstract

For a number of years, my colleagues and I (see acknowledgments) have been building, testing, and refining a diagrammatic environment (“Belvedere”) intended to support secondary school children’s learning of critical inquiry skills in the context of science (Suthers, Connelly, Lesgold, Paolucci, Toth, Toth, & Weiner, 2001; Toth, Suthers, & Lesgold, 2002). The diagrams were first designed to engage students in complex scientific argumentation with the help of an intelligent tutoring system. (For the purposes of this chapter, scientific argumentation is a dialectic in which participants mutually evaluate alternative hypotheses according to their consistency with empirical evidence and related criteria such as plausibility of the proposed causal explanations and reliability of the evidence. Participants may but need not necessarily take conflicting positions.) The diagrams were later simplified to focus on evidential relations between data and hypotheses. This change was driven in part by a refocus on collaborative learning (Koschmann, 1994; Slavin, 1980; Webb & Palincsar, 1996), which led to a major change in how we viewed the role of the interface representations. Rather than being a medium of communication or a formal record of the argumentation process, we came to view the representations as resources (stimuli and guides) for conversation and reasoning (Collins & Ferguson, 1993; Roschelle, 1994). Laboratory and field trials with Belvedere provided many examples of situations in which Belvedere’s diagrammatic representations appeared to be influencing learner’s argumentation. Meanwhile, various other projects with similar goals (i.e., critical inquiry in a collaborative learning context) were using substantially different representational systems (to be reviewed in this chapter). Finding that the literature lacked systematic research on this variable, I undertook a program of exploring the hypothesis that the expressive constraints imposed by a representation and the information (or lack of information) that it makes salient may have facilitative effects on students’ argumentation during collaborative learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Baker, M. (2003). Computer-mediated argumentative interactions for the co-elaboration of scientific notations. To appear in J. Andriessen, M. Baker, and D. Suthers (Eds.) Arguing to Learn: Confronting Cognitions in Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning Environments. Kluwer, 2003 (this volume).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, P. (1997, December). Using argument representations to make thinking visible for individuals and groups. In Proceedings of the Computer Supported Collaborative Learning Conference 97, 10–19. University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M.T.H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M., Reimann, P., and Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145–182, 1989.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H. and Brennan, S.E. (1991). Grounding in communication. In L.B. Resnick, J.M. Levine and S.D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition (pp. 127–149). (need city): American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. and Ferguson, W. (1993). Epistemic forms and epistemic games: Structures and strategies to guide inquiry. Educational Psychologist, 28 (1), 25–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conklin, J. and Begeman, M.L. (1987). gIBIS: A hypertext tool for team design deliberation. In Hypertext’87 Proceedings (247–252). New York: ACM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., and Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior.11, 671–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guzdial, M., Hmelo, C., Hubscher, R., Nagel, K., Newstetter, W., Puntambekar, S., Shabo, A., Turns, J., and Kolodner, J. L. (1997, December). Integrating and guiding collaboration: Lessons learned in computer-supported collaborative learning research at Georgia Tech. In Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Computer Supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL’97) (pp. 91–100 ). Toronto: University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harp, B. and Neches, R. (1988, March). Notecards: An everyday tool for aiding in complex tasks. In Proceedings of the Architectures for Intelligent Interfaces Workshop (pp. 287–304 ). Monterey, CA: ACM/SIGCHI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koschmann, T. D. (1994). Toward a theory of computer support for collaborative learning. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3 (3), 219–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotovsky, K. and H. A. Simon (1990). What makes some problems really hard: Explorations in the problem space of difficulty. Cognitive Psychology 22, 143–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H. and Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science 11 (1), 65–99. 1987.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lohse, G. L. (1997). Models of graphical perception. In M. Helander, T. K. Landauer, and P. Prabhu (Eds.), Handbook of Human-Computer Interaction (pp. 107–135 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier

    Google Scholar 

  • Science B.V. Norman, D. N. (1999). Affordance, conventions, and design. ACM interactions 6 (3), 38–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Novak, J. (1990). Concept mapping: A useful tool for science education. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 27 (10), 937–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Novick, L.R. and Hmelo, C.E. (1994). Transferring symbolic representations across nonisomorphic problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20 (6), 1296–1321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Okada, T. and Simon, H. A. (1997). Collaborative discovery in a scientific domain. Cognitive Science 21(2): 109–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, D. K., and Gomez, L. M. (1994). The collaboratory notebook: A distributed knowledge-building environment for project-enhanced learning. In Proceedings of Ed-Media `94,Charlottesville, VA: Association for the Advancement of Computing in Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puntambekar, S., Nagel, K., Hubscher, R., Guzdial, M., and Kolodner, J. (1997, December). Intra-group and intergroup: An exploration of learning with complementary collaboration tools. In Proceedings of the Computer Supported Collaborative Learning Conference `97 ( 207–214 ). Toronto: University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ranney, M., Schank, P., and Diehl, C. (1995). Competence versus performance in critical reasoning: Reducing the gap by using Convince Me. Psychology Teaching Review 4 (2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader, W. (1997). Structuring Argument: The Role of Constraint in the Explication of Scientific Argument. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roschelle, J. (1994, May). Designing for cognitive communication: Epistemic fidelity or mediating collaborative inquiry? The Arachnet Electronic Journal of Virtual Culture, 2 (2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G. (Ed.) (1993). Distributed cognitions: psychological and educational considerations. Cambridge, England; New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scardamalia, M., and Bereiter, C. (1991). Higher levels of agency for children in knowledge building: A challenge for the design of new knowledge media. The Journal of the Learning Sciences 1(1),37–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scardamalia, M., Bereiter, C., Brett, C., Burtis, P.J., Calhoun, C., and Smith Lea, N. (1992). Educational applications of a networked communal database. Interactive Learning Environments,2(1), 45–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. B. and Glassner, A. (2003). The blind and the paralytic: Supporting argumentation in everyday and scientific issues. To appear in J. Andriessen, M. Baker, and D. Suthers (Eds.) Arguing to Learn: Confronting Cognitions in Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning Environments. Kluwer, 2003 (this volume).

    Google Scholar 

  • Slavin, R. E. (1990). Cooperative learning: Theory, research, and practice. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky, P., Fox, B., King, R., and Lewis, C. (1987). Computer-aided reasoned discourse, or, how to argue with a computer. In R. Guindon (Ed.), Cognitive Science and Its Applications for Human-Computer Interaction (pp. 109–162 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, B. S., and Bransford, J. D. (1979). Constraints on effective elaboration: Effects of precision and subject generation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18, 769–777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stenning, K. and Oberlander, J. (1995). A cognitive theory of graphical and linguistic reasoning: Logic and implementation. Cognitive Science 19 (1), 97–140. 1995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stenning, K. and Yule, P. (1997). Image and language in human reasoning: A syllogistic illustration. Cognitive Psychology 34, 109–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suthers, D., Connelly, J., Lesgold, A., Paolucci, M., Toth, E., Toth, J., and Weiner, A. (2001). Representational and Advisory Guidance for Students Learning Scientific Inquiry. In Forbus, K. D., and Feltovich, P. J. (2001). Smart machines in education: The coming revolution in educational technology. Menlo Park, CA: AAAI/Mit Press, pp. 7–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suthers, D. and Hundhausen, C. (2003). An Empirical Study of the Effects of Representational Guidance on Collaborative Learning. Accepted to the Journal of the Learning Sciences for publication in 2003. Suthers, D., Hundhausen, C. and Girardeaue, L. (submitted). Comparing the roles of reprsentations in face- to-face and online computer supported collaborative learning. Submitted August 2003 to Computers in Education

    Google Scholar 

  • Suthers, D., Toth, E., and Weiner, A. (1997, December). An integrated approach to implementing collaborative inquiry in the classroom. In Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Computer Supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL’97) ( 272–279 ). Toronto: University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suthers, D. and Weiner, A. (1995, October). Groupware for developing critical discussion skills. CSCL `95, Computer Supported Cooperative Learning, Bloomington, Indiana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth, E., Suthers, D., and Lesgold, A. (in press). Mapping to know: The effects of evidence maps and reflective assessment on scientific inquiry skills. To appear in Science Education

    Google Scholar 

  • Utgoff, P. (1986). Shift of bias for inductive concept learning. In R. Michalski, J. Carbonell, T. Mitchell (Eds.) Machine Learning: An Artitificial Intelligence Approach, Volume II (pp. 107–148 ). Los Altos: Morgan Kaufmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Bruggen, J. M. and Kirschner, P. A. (2003). Designing external representations to support solving wicked problems. To appear in J. Andriessen, M. Baker, and D. Suthers (Eds.) Arguing to Learn: Confronting Cognitions in Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning Environments Kluwer, 2003 (this volume).

    Google Scholar 

  • Veerman, A. (2003). Constructive discussions through electronic dialogue. To appear in J. Andriessen, M. Baker, and D. Suthers (Eds.) Arguing to Learn: Confronting Cognitions in Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning Environments. Kluwer, 2003 (this volume).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wan, D., and Johnson, P. M. (1994, October). Experiences with CLARE: a Computer-supported collaborative learning environment. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 41, 851–879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, N. and Palincsar, A. (1996). Group processes in the classroom. In D. Berlmer and R. Calfee, (Eds.), Handbook of Educational Psychology New York: Simon and Schuster Macmillian.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. (1997). The nature of external representations in problem solving. Cognitive Science, 21 (2), 179–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2003 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Suthers, D.D. (2003). Representational Guidance for Collaborative Inquiry. In: Andriessen, J., Baker, M., Suthers, D. (eds) Arguing to Learn. Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, vol 1. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0781-7_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0781-7_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-6320-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-017-0781-7

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics