Skip to main content

Disruption of the Sense of Agency: From Perception to Self-knowldge

  • Chapter
Neuropsychology of the Sense of Agency

Abstract

Different planes of disruption may result in a loss or breakdown of the sense of agency. They may pertain to the different levels of perceptual, attentional, and psychiatric disorders, and generally they have in common a loss of the sense of control and a disruption of binding between intentions and actions. In some cases, they imply a deficit in the sense of ownership, which may be subdivided into ownership for body-actions and for thought-action. In other cases, what is lost is the sense of agency itself, similarly divided into agency for action and agency for thoughts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Baron-Cohen S (1997) Mindblindness. An Essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  2. Rossetti Y, Pisella L (2002) Several “vision for action” system: a guide for dissociating and integratine dorsal and ventral functions. In: Prinz W, Hommel B (eds) Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 62–119

    Google Scholar 

  3. De Vignemont F, Fourneret P (2004) The sense of agency: a philosophical and empirical review of the “Who” system. Conscious Cogn 13:1–19

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Della Sala S, Marchetti C, Spinnler H (1991) Right-sided anarchic (alien) hand: a longitudinal study. Neuropsychologia 29:1113–1127

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Riddoch JM (1990) Loss of visual imagery: a generation deficit. Cogn Neuropsychol 7:249–273

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Weiskrantz L (1989) Blindsight. In: Boller F, Grafman J (eds) Handbook of neuropsychology. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 375–385

    Google Scholar 

  7. Paillard J (2005) Vectorial versus configural encoding of body space: a neural basis for a distinction between body schema and body image. In: De Preester H, Knockaert V (eds) Body imagery and body schema. Interdisciplinary perspectives on the body. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 89–109

    Google Scholar 

  8. Rossetti Y, Gilles R, Farnè A, Rossetti A (2005) Implicit body representations in action. In: De Preester H, Knockaert V (eds) Body imagery and body schema. Interdisciplinary perspectives on the body. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 111–125

    Google Scholar 

  9. Rossetti Y, Revonsuo A (2000) Beyond dissociation: interaction between dissociated implicit and explicit processing. John Benjamins, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  10. Miltner WHR, Braun CH, Coles MGH (1997) Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-production task: evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. J Cognitive Neurosci 9:787–797

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Milner AD, McIntosh, RD (2003) Reaching between obstacles in spatial neglect and visual extinction. Prog in Brain Res 144:213–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Milner AD, McIntosh RD (2005) The neurological basis of visual neglect. Curr Opin Neurol 18:748–753

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Jeannerod M, Rossetti Y (1993) Visuomotor coordination as a dissociable function: experimental and clinical evidence. In: Kennard C (ed) Visual perceptual defects. Baillère’s Clinical Neurology, International Practise and Research. Ballière Tindall, London, pp 439–460

    Google Scholar 

  14. Vallar G (1997) Spatial frames of reference and somatosensory processing: a neuropsychological perspective. Philos T R Soc Lond B 352:1401–1409

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Vecchi T, Bottini G (2006) Imagery and spatial cognition. Method, models and cognitive assessment. John Benjamins, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  16. Bisiach E, Perani D, Vallar G, Bert A (1986) Unilateral neglect: personal and extrapersonal. Neuropsychologia 24:759–767

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Vallar G, Bottini G, Paulesu E (2003) Neglect syndromes: the role of parietal cortex. In: Siegel AM, Andersen RA, Freund H-J, Spencer DD (eds) Advances in neurology, vol. 93. The parietal lobes. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Philadelphia, pp 293-319

    Google Scholar 

  18. Pia L, Neppi-Madona M, Ricci R, Berti A (2004) The anatomy of anosognosia for emiplegia: a meta analysis. Cortex 40:367–377

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Blass DM, Hatanpaa KJ, Brandt J et al (2004) Dementia in hippocampal sclerosis resembles frontotemporal dementia more than Alzheimer disease. Neurology 63:492–497

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1995) Infant’s understanding of people and things: from body imitation to folk psychology. In: Bermudez JL, Marcel A, Eilan N (eds) The body and the self. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 43–69

    Google Scholar 

  21. Gallese V (2001) The “Shared Manifold” hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy. J Consciousness Stud 8:33–50

    Google Scholar 

  22. Georgieff N, Jeannerod M (1998) Beyond consciousness of external reality: a “Who” system for consciousness of action and self-consciousness. Conscious Cogn 7:465–477

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Frith CD, Done DJ (1989) Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action. Psychol Med 19:359–363

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Jeannerod M, Farrer C, Franck N et al (2003) Recognition of action in normal and schizophrenic subjects. In: Kircher T, David A (eds) The self in neuroscience and psychiatry. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 380–406

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  25. Davies M, Coltheart M, Langdon R, Breen N (2001) Monothematic delusions: towards a two-factor account. In: On understanding and explaining schizophrenia, a special issue of Philosophy. Psychiatry and Psychology 8:133–158

    Google Scholar 

  26. Blakemore S-J, Sarfati Y, Bazin N, Decety J (2003) The detection of intentional contingencies in simple animations in patients with delusions of persecution. Psychol Med 33: 1433–1441

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Newcombe N (1989) Development of spatial perspective taking. In: Reese HW (ed) Advances in child development and behavior, vol. 22, pp 203–247

    Google Scholar 

  28. Meltzoff AN, Decety J (2003) What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philos T Roy Soc B 358:491–500

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Gallagher S (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci 4:14–21

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Frith U, Frith CD (1999) Interacting minds. A biological basis. Science 286:1692–1695

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Blakemore S-J, Rees G, Frith CD (1998) How do we predict the consequences of our actions? A functional imaging study. Neuropsychologia 36:521–529

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. David N, Gawronski A, Santos NS et al (2007) Dissociation between key processes of social cognition in autism: impaired mentalizing but intact sense of agency. J Autism Dev Disord 38:593–605

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Daprati E, Wriessnegger S, Lacquaniti F (2007) Kinematic cues and recognition of self-generated actions. Exp Brain Res 177:31–44

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Ramnani N, Miall RC (2004) A system in the human brain for predicting the actions of the others. Nat Neurosci 7:85–90

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Synofzik M, Vosgerau G, Newen A (2008) Beyond the comparator model: a multifactorial two-step account of agency. Conscious Cogn 17:219–239

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Cole ME, Frost RO, Heimberg RG, Rhéaume J (2003) “Not just right experiences”: perfectionism, obsessive-compulsive features and general psychopathology. Behav Res Ther 41:681–700

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Vallacher RR, Wegner DM (1989) Levels of personal agency: individual variation in action identification. J Pers Soc Psychol 57:660–671

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Vallacher RR, Wegner DM (1985) A theory of action identification. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  39. Boyer P, Liénard P (2006) Why ritualized behavior? Prediction systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behav Brain Sci 29:1–56

    Google Scholar 

  40. Rufer M, Fricke S, Held D et al (2006) Dissociation and symptom dimensions of obsessivecompulsive disorder: a replication study. Eur Arch Psy Clin N 256:146–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Balconi M, Crivelli D (2010) FRN and P300 ERP effect modulation in response to feedback sensitivity: the contribution of punishment-reward system (BIS/BAS) and behaviour Identification of action. Neurosci Res 66:162–172

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Gehring WJ, Fencsik DE (2001) Functions of the medial frontal cortex in the processing of conflict and errors. J Neurosci 21:9430–9437

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Holroyd CB, Krigolson OE, Baker R et al (2009) When is an error not a prediction error? An electrophysiological investigation. Cogn Affect Behav Ne 9:59–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L (1990) Effects of errors in choice reaction tasks on the ERP under focused and divided attention. In: Brunia CHM, Gaillard AWK, Kok A (eds) Psychophysiological brain research. Tilburg University Press, Tilburg, Netherlands, pp 192–195

    Google Scholar 

  45. Luu P, Flaisch T, Tucker DM (2000) Medial frontal cortex in action monitoring. J Neurosci 20:464–469

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Balconi M, Crivelli D (2009) Spatial and temporal cognition for the sense of agency: neuropsychological evidences. Cogn Process 10:182–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Heldmann M, Russeler J, Munte T (2008) Internal and external information in error processing. BMC Neurosci 9:1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Miltner WHR, Braun CH, Coles MGH (1997) Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. J Cognitive Neurosci 9:788–798

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Ehlis A-C, Herrmann MJ, Bernhard A, Fallgatter AJ (2005) Monitoring of internal and external error signals. J Psychophysiol 19:263–269

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Müller SV, Möller J, Rodriguez-Fornells A, Münte TF (2005) Brain potentials related to selfgenerated and external information used for performance monitoring. Clin Neurophysiol 116:63–74

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Vocat R, Pourtois G, Vuilleumier P (2008) Unavoidable errors: a spatio-temporal analysis of time-course and neural sources of evoked potentials associated with error processing in a speeded task. Neuropsychologia 46:2545–2555

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Dikman ZV, Allen JJB (2000) Error monitoring during reward and avoidance learning in high-and low-socialized individuals. Psychophysiology 37:43–54

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Gray JA (1981) A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In: Eysenck HJ (ed) A model for personality. Springer, Berlin, pp 246–277

    Google Scholar 

  54. Carver CS, White TL (1994) Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 67:319–333

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Gray JA (1990) Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cognition Emotion 4:269–288

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Gray JA (1982) The neuropsychology of anxiety: an inquiry into the functions of the septohippocampal system. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  57. Fowles DC (1980) The three arousal model: implications of Gray’s two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy. Psychophysiology 17:87–104

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Boksem MAS, Tops M, Wester AE et al (2006) Error-related ERP components and individual differences in punishment and reward sensitivity. Brain Res 1101:92–101

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Holroyd CB, Coles MGH (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109:679–709

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Vallacher RR, Nowak A, Markus J, Strauss J (1998) Dynamics in the coordination of mind and action. In: Kofta M, Weary G, Sedek G (eds) Personal control in action: cognitive and motivational mechanisms. Plenum, New York, pp 27–59

    Google Scholar 

  61. Wegner DM, Vallacher RR, Macomber G et al (1984) The emergence of action. J Pers Soc Psychol 46:269–279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Vallacher RR, Wegner DM, Frederick J (1987) The presentation of self through action identification. Soc Cognition 5:301–322

    Google Scholar 

  63. Wegner DM, Vallacher RR, Kiersted GW, Dizadji DM (1986) Action identification in the emergence of social behavior. Soc Cognition 4:18–38

    Google Scholar 

  64. Rotter JB (1966) Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol Monogr-Gen A 80:1–28

    Google Scholar 

  65. Snyder M, Campbell BH (1982) Self-monitoring: the self in action. In: Suls J (ed) Psychological perspectives on the self. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp 185–207

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer-Verlag Italia

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Balconi, M. (2010). Disruption of the Sense of Agency: From Perception to Self-knowldge. In: Michela, B. (eds) Neuropsychology of the Sense of Agency. Springer, Milano. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-88-470-1587-6_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics