Skip to main content

A Multistage Learning Model for Cultural Transmission: Evidence from Three Indigenous Societies

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Social Learning and Innovation in Contemporary Hunter-Gatherers

Abstract

Based on generational differences and social relations, researchers have hypothesized that the transmission of cultural knowledge occurs through at least three different, not mutually exclusive, paths: (1) parents (vertical), (2) age peers (horizontal), and (3) elders (oblique). Here we contribute to this body of research by presenting three case studies showing evidence of a multistage model of cultural learning in which vertical transmission in childhood loses preeminence toward horizontal and oblique models as subjects’ age. The first case study documents and analyzes Baka children's daily activities (southeast Cameroon) in an attempt to understand (1) how time investments might affect the acquisition of knowledge and (2) the importance of scaffolding on knowledge acquisition. Building on this idea, the second case study explores the transmission of knowledge through the life cycle, documenting the accumulation of knowledge required for collecting wild honey among children and adults from a Jenu Kuruba tribal community in South India. The last case study uses data from the Tsimane’ (Bolivian Amazon) to analyze the correspondence between levels of adult knowledge and the knowledge of (a) the same-sex parent, (b) age peers, and (c) parental cohort. Results from this study suggest that – at adulthood – cultural knowledge is most likely a mix of information gathered from a variety of sources. Overall, the three case studies give evidence to support the multistage learning model for cultural transmission but also emphasize the importance of social learning during childhood, a period during which individuals acquire the baseline knowledge that allow the latter development of complex skills through scaffolding and the integration of information from multiple models.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Researchers differentiate between modes of cultural transmission and processes of social learning (Hewlett et al. 2011). Modes of transmission refer to categories of individuals (i.e., parents, elders, peers, the prestigious, the majority) who are sources of information. Processes of social learning include teaching, emulation, imitation, and collaborative learning. In this article we mainly deal with modes of transmission and use “copy” to refer to all social learning processes.

  2. 2.

    “Scheduled” here refers to historically disadvantaged castes and tribes recognized by the Indian constitution.

  3. 3.

    A day’s wages from laboring on local coffee estates is worth about 1–1.5 kg of forest honey; hives produce 1–30 kg depending on the bee species.

  4. 4.

    All theoretical LEK questions were pretested in an independent sample for clarity and to achieve a mix of difficulties of responses.

References

  • Althabe G (1965) Changements sociaux chez les Pygmées baka de l’Est-Cameroun. Cah d’Etud Afr 5(20):561–592

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger R (2000) The life history of culture learning in a face-to-face society. Ethos 28(2):1–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Bahuchet S (1991) Les Pygmées d’aujourd’hui en Afrique centrale. J Afr 61(1):5–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey RC, Bahuchet S, Hewlett BS (1992) Development in the Central African rainforest: concern for forest peoples. In: Cleaver K, Munasinghe M, Dyson M, Egli N, Peuker A, Wencelius F (eds) Conservation of West and Central African rainforest/Conservation de la forêt dense en Afrique centrale et de l’Ouest. World Bank, Washington, DC, pp 202–211

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkes F, Colding J, Folke C (2000) Rediscovery of traditional ecological knowledge as adaptive management. Ecol App 10(5):1251–1262

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird DW, Bliege Bird R (2005) Mardu children’s hunting strategies in the Western Desert, Australia: foraging and the evolution of human life histories. In: Hewlett B, Lamb ME (eds) Hunter gatherer childhoods. Aldine de Gruyter, New York, pp 129–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson P (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson P (2005) The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Silk JB (2006) How humans evolved. Norton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW, Chen KH, Dornbusch SM (1982) Theory and observation in cultural transmission. Science 218(4567):19–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins A, Brown JS, Newman SE (1989) Cognitive apprenticeship: teaching the crafts of reading, writing, and mathematics. In: Resnick B (ed) Knowing, learning, and instruction: essays in honor of Robert Glaser. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, pp 453–494

    Google Scholar 

  • Demps K, Zorondo-Rodriguez, García C, Reyes-García V (2012a) The selective persistence of local ecological knowledge: honey collecting with the Jenu Kuruba. Hum Ecol 40:427–434

    Google Scholar 

  • Demps K, Zorondo-Rodriguez F, García C, Reyes-García V (2012b) Social learning across the lifecycle: cultural knowledge acquisition for honey hunting among the Jenu Kuruba, India. Evol Hum Behav 33(5):460–470

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis R (1996) A taste for movement: an exploration of the social ethics of the Tsimane’ of lowland Bolivia. Dissertation, St Andrews University, Scotland

    Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M, Ghirlanda S (2007) Evolution of imitation does not explain the origin of human cumulative culture. J Theor Biol 246:129–135

    Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M, Eriksson K, Ghirlanda S (2007) Critical social learning: a solution to Rogers’ paradox of non-adaptive culture. Am Anthropol 109:727–734

    Google Scholar 

  • Geissler P, Harris SA, Prince R, Olsen A, Odhiambo RA, Oketch-Rabah H et al (2002) Medicinal plants used by Luo mothers and children in Bondo district, Kenya. J Ethnopharmacol 83(1–2):39–54

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven M, Kaplan H, Gutierrez M (2006) How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the correct of delayed growth. J Hum Evol 51:454–470

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagino I, Yamauchi T (2013) Gender-age differences in daily time-place use and activity pattern of African rainforest forager’s children in Cameroon. Paper presented at the 10th Conference on Hunting and Gathering Societies – CHaGS, University of Liverpool, UK, 25–28 June 2013

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris J (1999) The nurture assumption: why children turn out the way they do. Bloomsbury, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Broesch J (2011) On the nature of cultural transmission networks: evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philos Trans R Soc 366:1139–1148

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Gil-White FJ (2001) The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evol Hum Behav 22:165–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, McElreath R (2003) The evolution of cultural evolution. Evol Anthropol 12(3):123–135

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2008) Five misunderstandings about cultural evolution. Hum Nat 19(2):119–137

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS, Cavalli-Sforza LL (1986) Cultural transmission among Aka Pygmies. Am Anthropol 88:922–934

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS, Fouts HN, Boyette AH, Hewlett BL (2011) Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366(1567):1168–1178

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunn ES (2002) Evidence for the precocious acquisition of plant knowledge by Zapotec children. In: Stepp JR, Wyndham FS, Zarger R (eds) Ethnobiology and biocultural diversity. International Society of Ethnobiology, Athens, pp 604–613

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamei N (2005) Play among Baka Children in Cameroon. In: Hewlett BS, Lamb ME (eds) Hunter-gatherer childhoods: evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives. Aldine Transaction, New Brunswick, pp 343–359

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline M, Boyd R, Henrich J (2013) Teaching and the life history of cultural transmission in Fijian villages. Hum Nat 24:351–374

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN (2004) Social learning strategies. Learn Behav 32(1):4–14

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy DF (1996) Playing on the mother ground: cultural routines for children's development. Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy D (1999) Playing on the mother-ground: cultural routines for children’s development. Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonti M (2011) The future is written: impact of scripts on the cognition, selection, knowledge and transmission of medicinal plant use and its implications for ethnobotany and ethnopharmacology. J Ethnopharmacol 134(3):542–555. doi:10.1016/j.jep.2011.01.017

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lozada M, Ladio AH, Weigandt M (2006) Cultural transmission of ethnobotanical knowledge in a rural community of northwestern Patagonia, Argentina. Econ Bot 60(4):374–385

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard AE, Tovote KE (2010) Learning from other children. In: Lancy CF, Bock J, Gaskins S (eds) The anthropology of learning in childhood. Alta Mira Press, New York, pp 181–199

    Google Scholar 

  • McElreath R, Strimling P (2008) When natural selection favors imitation of parents. Curr Anthropol 49(2):307–316

    Google Scholar 

  • Merrill DC, Reiser BJ, Ranney M, Trafton JG (1992) Effective tutoring techniques: a comparison of human tutors and intelligent tutoring systems. J Learn Sci 2:277–305

    Google Scholar 

  • Morelli GA, Rogoff B, Angelillo C (2003) Cultural variation in young children’s access to work or involvement in specialised child-focused activities. Int J Behav Dev 27(3):264–274

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohmagari K, Berkes F (1997) Transmission of indigenous knowledge and bush skills among the western James Bay Cree Women of Subarctic Canada. Hum Ecol 25(2):197–222

    Google Scholar 

  • Perreault C, Cristina M, Boyd R (2012) A Bayesian approach to the evolution of social learning. Evol Hum Behav 33:449–459

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince RJ, Geissler PW, Nokes K, Maende JO, Okatcha F, Sternberg R (2001) Knowledge of herbal and pharmaceutical medicines among Luo children in western Kenya. Anthropol Med 8(2–3):211–235

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-García V, Molina JL, Broesch J, Calvet L, Huanca T, Saus J et al (2008) Do the aged and knowledgeable men enjoy more prestige? A test of predictions from the prestige-bias model of cultural transmission. Evol Hum Behav 29(4):275–281

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-García V, Ledezma JC, Paneque-Galvez J, Orta M, Gueze M, Lobo A et al (2012a) Presence and purpose of non-indigenous peoples on indigenous lands: a descriptive account from the Bolivian Lowlands. Soc Nat Resour 25(3):270–284

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-García V, Orta-Martínez M, Gueze M, Luz AC, Paneque-Galvez J, Macia MJ et al (2012b) Does participatory mapping increase conflict? A randomized experimental evaluation in the Bolivian Amazon. Appl Geogr 34:650–658

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-García V, Luz AC, Gueze M, Paneque-Gálvez J, Macia M, Orta-Martínez M et al (2013) Secular trends on traditional ecological knowledge: an analysis of different domains of knowledge among Tsimane’ men. Learn Individ Differ 27:206–212

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson P, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff B (1986) Adult assistance of children’s learning. In: Raphael TE (ed) The contexts of school-based literacy. Random House, New York, pp 27–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz-Mallen I, Morsello C, Reyes-García V, Barros Marcondes R (2013) Children use of time and traditional ecological learning in the Amazon: a case study in two Brazilian indigenous societies. Learn Individ Differ 27:213–222

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumann S, Macinko S (2007) Subsistence in coastal fisheries policy: what's in a word? Mar Policy 31(6):706–718. doi:10.1016/j.marpol.2006.12.010

  • Stross B (1973) Acquisition of botanical terminology by Tzeltal children. In: Edmonson MS (ed) Meaning in Mayan languages. Mouton, The Hague, pp 107–141

    Google Scholar 

  • Vadez V, Reyes-García V, Huanca T, Leonard WR (2008) Cash cropping, farm technologies, and deforestation: what are the connections? A model with empirical data from the Bolivian Amazon. Hum Organ 67(4):384–396

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygostky LS (1978) Mind and society: development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood D, Bruner J, Ross G (1976) The role of tutoring in problem solving. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip 17:89–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Zarger R (2002) Acquisition and transmission of subsistence knowledge by Q’eqchi’ Maya in Belize. In: Stepp JR, Wyndham FS, Zarger R (eds) Ethnobiology and biocultural diversity. International Society of Ethnobiology, Athens, pp 592–603

    Google Scholar 

  • Zarger R, Stepp JR (2004) Persistence of botanical knowledge among Tzeltal Maya Children. Curr Anthropol 45:413–418

    Google Scholar 

  • Zarger RK (2010) Learning the environment. In: Lancy D, Bock J, Gaskins S (eds) The anthropology of learning in childhood. AltaMira Press, Walnut Creek, pp 341–369

    Google Scholar 

  • Zent S (2009) Traditional Ecological Knowledge (TEK) and biocultural diversity: a close-up look at linkages, delearning trends and changing patterns of transmission. In Bates P, Chiba M, Kube S, Nakashima D (eds) Learning & knowing in indigenous societies today. UNESCO, Paris

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Research was funded by grants from the Cultural Anthropology Programs, NSF (BCS-0322380 for the Tsimane’ case study and BCS-0726612 for the Jenu Kuruba case study), and the European Research Council, Starting Grant ERC (FP7-261971-LEK), for the case study on the Baka. We thank the Crop Physiology Lab at International Crop Research Institute for the SemiArid Tropics-Patancheru for providing office facilities to Reyes-García. Thanks also go to the Baka, the Jenu Kuruba, and the Tsimane’ for their patience, friendship, and continuous support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Victoria Reyes-García .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Japan

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Reyes-García, V., Gallois, S., Demps, K. (2016). A Multistage Learning Model for Cultural Transmission: Evidence from Three Indigenous Societies. In: Terashima, H., Hewlett, B.S. (eds) Social Learning and Innovation in Contemporary Hunter-Gatherers. Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series. Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_4

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Tokyo

  • Print ISBN: 978-4-431-55995-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-4-431-55997-9

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics