Skip to main content

Determinants of Cultural Evolutionary Rates

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Dynamics of Learning in Neanderthals and Modern Humans Volume 1

Part of the book series: Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series ((RNMH))

Abstract

The cultural Moran model is a simple stochastic model of birth, social learning, and death in a finite population, which assumes that one individual is born at a time, who then engages in social learning from the older individuals of the population, followed by the death of one individual other than the newborn. Using this model, we propose two different theoretical definitions for the cultural evolutionary rate. The first applies to the successive fixation of many discrete cultural traits, and the second to the change of one continuous cultural trait. Taking the case of random oblique transmission (a randomly chosen older individual is copied) as the baseline, we compare the effects of greater innovativeness and increased population size on the cultural evolutionary rate. With individuals capable of direct bias (a particular variant of a cultural trait is preferred and an older individual carrying that variant is identified and copied), the innovation rate is shown to be at least as important as—and in some cases much more so than—the population size in determining the cultural evolutionary rate. Moreover, the cultural evolutionary rate is predicted to increase as the number of acquaintances from whom social learning can occur increases, with the possible implication that a cultural trait that is normally acquired early in life may evolve more slowly than one that is normally acquired later. In addition, we show that one-to-many transmission (one older individual serves as the teacher to many novices) does not in itself have any effect on the cultural evolutionary rate. However, when the teacher is more innovative than others, increased population size has a small decelerating effect.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aoki K, Feldman MW (2014) Evolution of learning strategies in temporally and spatially variable environments: a review of theory. Theor Popul Biol, in press

    Google Scholar 

  • Aoki K, Lehmann L, Feldman MW (2011) Rates of cultural change and patterns of cultural accumulation in stochastic models of social transmission. Theor Popul Biol 79:192–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banks WE, d’Errico F, Peterson AT, Kageyama M, Sima A, Sánchez-Goñi M-F (2008) Neanderthal extinction by competitive exclusion. PLoS One 3:1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birdsell JB (1957) Some population problems involving Pleistocene man. Cold Spring Harbor Symp Quant Biol 22:47–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown MJ, Feldman MW (2009) Sociocultural epistasis and cultural exaptation in footbinding, marriage form, and religious practices in early 20th century Taiwan. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:22139–22144

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Chudek M, Heller S, Birch S, Henrich J (2012) Prestige-biased cultural learning: bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evol Hum Behav 33:46–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico F, Stringer CB (2011) Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Phil Trans R Soc B 366:1060–1069

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond J (1997) Guns, germs, and steel. Norton, NewYork

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (1993) Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behav Brain Sci 16:681–735

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eerkens JW, Lipo CP (2007) Cultural transmission theory and the archaeological record: providing context to understanding variation and temporal changes in material culture. J Archaeol Res 15:239–274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ewens WJ (2004) Mathematical population genetics, 2nd edn. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman MW, Aoki K, Kumm J (1996) Individual versus social learning: evolutionary analysis in a fluctuating environment. Anthropol Sci 104:209–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gale JS (1990) Theoretical population genetics. Unwin Hyman, London

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons A (2011) Ancient footprints tell tales of travel. Science 332:534–535

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green RE, Krause J, Briggs AW et al (2010) A draft sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science 328:71–722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guglielmino CR, Viganotti C, Hewlett B, Cavalli-Sforza LL (1995) Cultural variation in Africa: role of mechanisms of transmission and adaptation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 92:7585–7589

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton MJ, Milne BT, Walker RS, Burger O, Brown JH (2007) The complex structure of hunter-gatherer social networks. Proc R Soc B 274:2195–2202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J (2004) Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses—the Tasmanian case. Am Antiquity 69:197–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Broesch J (2011) On the nature of cultural transmission networks: evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Phil Trans R Soc B 336:1139–1148

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS, Cavalli-Sforza LL (1986) Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. Am Anthropol 88:922–934

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett B, van de Koppel JMH, Cavalli-Sforza LL (1982) Exploration ranges of Aka pygmies of the Central African Republic. Man (NS) 17:418–430

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett BS, Fouts HN, Boyette AH, Hewlett BL (2011) Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Phil Trans R Soc B 366:1168–1178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill KR (2012) Social structure and inter-group interactions in hunter-gatherers: how humans achieved cumulative culture. In: the Wenner-Gren foundations international symposium “cultural evolution—patterns of cultural change and diversification”

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill KR, Walker RS, Božičević M, Eder J, Headland T, Hewlett B, Hurtado AM, Marlowe F, Wiessner P, Wood B (2011) Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science 331:1286–1289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ichikawa M (1978) The residential groups of the Mbuti pygmies. Senri Ethnol Stud 1:131–188

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan P, Shennan S (2009) Diversity in hunter-gatherer technological traditions: mapping trajectories of cultural ‘descent with modification’ in northeast California. J Anthropol Archaeol 28:342–365

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura M (1969) The number of heterozygous nucleotide sites maintained in a finite population due to steady flux of mutations. Genetics 61:893–903

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimura M (1983) The neutral theory of molecular evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi K, Aoki K (2012) Innovativeness, population size and cumulative cultural evolution. Theor Popul Biol 82:38–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn SL (2012) Emergent patterns of creativity and innovation in early technologies. In: Elias S (ed) Origins of human innovation and creativity. Elsevier, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Lalueza-Fox C, Rosas A, Estalrrich A et al (2011) Genetic evidence for patrilocal mating behavior among Neandertal groups. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:250–253

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann L, Aoki K, Feldman MW (2011) On the number of independent cultural traits carried by individuals and populations. Phil Trans R Soc B 366:424–435

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lycett SJ, Gowlett AJ (2008) On questions surrounding the Acheulean ‘tradition’. World Archaeol 40:295–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe FW (2005) Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evol Anthropol 14:54–67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellars P, French JC (2011) Tenfold population increase in western Europe at the Neandertal—to—modern human transition. Science 333:623–627

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2009) How cultural evolutionary theory can inform social psychology and vice versa. Psychol Rev 116:929–952

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2011) Variable cultural acquisition costs constrain cumulative cultural evolution. PLoS One 6:1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mithen S (1996) The prehistory of the mind. Thames and Hudson, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran PAP (1958) Random processes in genetics. Proc Camb Phil Soc 54:60–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neubauer S, Hublin J-J (2012) The evolution of human brain development. Evol Biol 39:568–586

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien MJ, Lyman RL, Mesoudi A, VanPool TL (2010) Cultural traits as units of analysis. Phil Trans R Soc B 365:3797–3806

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pigeot N (1990) Technical and social actors flintknapping specialists and apprentices at Magdalenian Etiolles. Archeol Rev Camb 9:126–41

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG (2009) Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324:1298–1301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Read D (2006) Tasmanian knowledge and skill: maladaptive imitation or adequate technology? Am Antiquity 71:164–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers AR (1988) Does biology constrain culture? Am Anthropol 90: 819–831

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers DS, Ehrlich PR (2008) Natural selection and cultural rates of change. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:3416–3420

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers DS, Feldman MW, Ehrlich PR (2009) Inferring population histories using cultural data. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B 276:3835–3843

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan SJ, Steele J (1999) Cultural learning in hominids: a behavioural ecological approach. In: Box HO, Gibson KR (eds) Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 367–388

    Google Scholar 

  • Sørenson B (2011) Demography and the extinction of European Neanderthals. J Anthropol Archaeol 30:17–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart JR, Stringer CB (2012) Human evolution out of Africa: the role of refugia and climate change. Science 335:1317–1321

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strimling P, Sjöstrand J, Enquist M, Eriksson K (2009) Accumulation of independent cultural traits. Theor Popul Biol 76:77–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Phil Trans R Soc B 364:2405–2415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1999) The human adaptation for culture. Annu Rev Anthropol 28:509–529

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zilhão J, Angelucci DE, Badal-Garcia E et al (2010) Symbolic use of marine shell and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:1023–1028

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank Marc Feldman for helpful comments. This research was supported in part by Monbukagakusho grant 22101004.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kenichi Aoki .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Japan

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Aoki, K. (2013). Determinants of Cultural Evolutionary Rates. In: Akazawa, T., Nishiaki, Y., Aoki, K. (eds) Dynamics of Learning in Neanderthals and Modern Humans Volume 1. Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series. Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54511-8_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics