Skip to main content

Talking About Reasoning: How Important Is the Peer in Peer Collaboration?

  • Chapter
Discourse, Tools and Reasoning

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((NATO ASI F,volume 160))

Abstract

Using data from two studies of scientific reasoning, this chapter explores whether transactive discussion is the basis of productive peer collaborations and questions what role the partner plays in the apparent effectiveness of this type of discussion. In the first study, dyads who engaged in transactive discussion showed more improvement than dyads who did not have transactive discussions. In the second study, both dyads and children working alone showed improvement related to talk in general. However, dyads produced more transactive types of talk and showed a more complex understanding of the problem that they generated more quickly. Having a partner was not a necessary or sufficient condition for producing transactive talk but increased likelihood that it would occur. The data from these studies suggest that the value of peer collaborations may be that the presence of a partner provides a natural context for elaborating one’s own reasoning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Azmitia, M., & Montgomery, R. (1993). Friendship, transactive dialogues, and the development of scientific reasoning. Social Development, 2, 202–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, M. W. (Ed.). (1985). New directions for child development: Peer conflict and psychological growth. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, M. W., & Gibbs, J. C. (1983). Measuring the development of features of moral discussion. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 399–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bivens, J. A., & Berk, L. E. (1990). A longitudinal study of the development of elementary school children’s private speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 443–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. R. (1961). Cognitive tuning a factor affecting impression formation. Journal of Personality, 29, 235–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damon, W., & Killen, M. (1982). Peer interaction and the process of change in children’s moral reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 347–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damon, W., & Phelps, E. (1989). Critical distinctions among three approaches to peer education. International Journal of Education Research, 13, 9–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, J. H., Stasser, G., Spitzer, C. E., & Holt, R. W. (1976). Changes in group members’ preferences during discussion: An illustration with mock juries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 1177–1187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diaz, R. M., (1992). Methodological concerns in the study of private speech. In R. M. Diaz & L. E. Berk (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to s elf-regulation (pp. 55–81). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diaz, R. M., & Berk, L. E. (Eds.). (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self- regulation . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, K., & Klahr, D. (1989). Developmental differences in scientific discovery strategies. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 109–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, S., & Gauvain, M. (1992). Social and cultural influences on children’s collaborative interactions. In L. T. Winegar & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Children’s development within social contexts (pp. 155–180). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 215–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M., & Rogoff, B. (1989). Collaborative problem solving and children’s problem solving skills. Developmental Psychology, 25, 139–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H., & St. Clair, R. N. (Eds.). (1985). Recent advances in language, communication, and social psychology. New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., Dunbar, K., & Fay, A. L. (1990). Designing good experiments to test “bad” hypotheses. In J. Schräger & P. Langley (Eds.), Computational models of discovery and theory information. San Mateo, CA: Morgan-Kaufman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., Fay, A., & Dunbar, K. (1993). Heuristics for scientific experimentation: A developmental study. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 111–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, A. C. (1992). The effect of peer and adult-child transactive discussions on moral reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 191–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, A. C., & Tomasello, M. (1986). Transactive discussions with peers and adults. Developmental Psychology, 22, 681–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Brannock, J. (1977). Development of the isolation of variables scheme in experimental and “natural experiment” contexts. Developmental Psychology, 13, 9–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., Amsel, E., & O’Laughlin, M. (1988). The development of scientific thinking skills. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Light, P., & Perret-Clermont, A.-N. (1989). Social context effects in learning and testing. In A. Gellantly, D. Rogers, & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Cognition and social worlds (pp. 99–112). Oxford, England: Claredon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. (Original work published, 1932).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pontecorvo, C. (1987). Discussing for reasoning: The role of argument in knowledge construction. In E. DeCorte, H. Lodewijks, R. Parmentier, & P. Span (Eds.), Learning and instruction: A publication of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction. (Vol. 1, pp. 71–82). Oxford, England: Leuven University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pontecorvo, C., & Girardet, H. (1993). Arguing and reasoning in understanding historical topics. Cognition and Instruction, 11, 365–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 31–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1991). Conversation analysis and socially shared cognition. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 150–171). Washington, DC: APA Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sproull, L., & Keisler, S. (1991). Connections: New ways of working in networked organizations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasley, S. D. (1995). The role of talk in children’s peer collaborations. Developmental Psychology, 3, 207–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teasley, S. D., & Resnick, L. B. (1993). Social influences and cognitive change: Peer collaboration in a causal reasoning task. Unpublished manuscript, University of Pittsburgh, Learning Research and Development Center, Pittsburgh, PA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasley, S. D., & Roschelle, J. (1993) The construction of shared knowledge in collaborative problem solving. In S. Lajoie & S. Derry (Eds.), Computers as cognitive tools (pp. 229–258). Hillsdale, NJ : Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Accountability and complexity of thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 74–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tudge, J. (1992). Processes and consequences of peer collaboration: A Vygotskian analysis. Child Development, 63, 1364–1379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V., & Stone, C. A. (1985). The concept of internalization in Vygotsky’s account of the genesis of higher mental functions. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 162–179). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1960). The process of cognitive tuning in communication. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 61, 159–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1997 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Teasley, S.D. (1997). Talking About Reasoning: How Important Is the Peer in Peer Collaboration?. In: Resnick, L.B., Säljö, R., Pontecorvo, C., Burge, B. (eds) Discourse, Tools and Reasoning. NATO ASI Series, vol 160. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-03362-3_16

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-03362-3_16

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-08337-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-03362-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics