Skip to main content

Conflict as a Cognitive Schema: Toward a Social Cognitive Analysis of Conflict and Conflict Termination

  • Chapter
The Social Psychology of Intergroup Conflict

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Social Psychology ((SSSOC))

Abstract

An essential element in the make-up of conflicts is the subjective knowledge the parties hold concerning their relation. Such knowledge determines, first, whether the situation is characterized as a conflict, and, second, how the conflict is reacted to, affectively and behaviorally. This chapter looks at intergroup and international conflicts from the perspective of a theory of lay epistemology (cf. Bar-Tal & Bar-Tal, in press; Kruglanski, 1980; Kruglanski & Ajzen, 1983; Kruglanski & Jaffe, 1986; Kruglanski & Klar, 1985) dealing with the process of knowledge acquisition. According to this perspective, conflict is viewed as a specific content of knowledge, or as a specific cognitive schema. The specific content of knowledge contained in a conflict schema refers to incompatibility of goals between parties. A conflict situation is said to occur when at least one of the parties subscribes to the conflict schema. Thus, the retention or modification of the conflict schema may determine whether conflict is maintained or resolved.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Asch, S.E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 88–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Tal, D. (1984). Israeli-Palestinian conflict: A cognitive analysis. Unpublished manuscript, Tel-Aviv University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Tal, D., Bar-Tal, Y. (in press). New perspective of social psychology. In D. Bar-Tal A. Kruglanski (Eds.), The social psychology of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Tal, D., Geva, N. (1985). A cognitive basis of international conflict. In W. G. Austin S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations ( 2nd ed. ). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckmann, J. Kuhl, J. (1984). Altering information to gain action control: Functional aspects of human information processing in decision making. Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 224–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L. (1962). Aggression: A social psychological analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L., LePage, A. (1967). Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 202–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, J. (1964). Allowing for Soviet perceptions. In R. Fisher (Ed.), International conflict and behavioral science. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahrendorf, R (1959). Class and class conflicts in industrial society. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict: Constructive and destructive processes. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, M.J. (1962). Conceptual structure and group processes in an international simulation: Part 1. The perception of simulated nations (ONR Technical report No. 9 ). Princeton: Princeton University and Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eiser, J. R., Tajfel, H. (1972). Acquisition of information in dyadic interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 340–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finlay, D.J., Holsti, O. R., Fagen, R. R. (1967). Enemies in politics. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. (1969). International conflict for beginners. New York: Harper Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R., Ury, W. (1981). Getting to yes: Negotiating agreement without giving in. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R.J. (1983). Third party consultation as a method of intergroup conflict resolution: A review of studies. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 27, 301–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social cognition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frenkel-Brunswick, E. (1949). Intolerance of ambiguity as emotional and perceptual personality disposition. Journal of Personality, 18, 103–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freund, T., Kruglanski, A. W., Schpitzajzen, A. (1985). The freezing and unfreezing of impressional primacy: Effects of the need for structure and the fear of invalidity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 479–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamson, W. A. (1968). Power and discontent. Homewood, IL: Dorsey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, D. L. (1979). A cognitive-attributional analysis of stereotyping. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 12 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heradstveit, D. (1981). The Arab-Israeli conflict ( 2nd ed. ). Oslo: Universitetsforlaget.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E.T., Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social cognition and social perception. Annual Review of Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E.T., Bargh, J. A., Lombardi, W. (1985). The nature of priming effects on categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 59–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holsti, O. R. (1972). Crisis, escalation, war. Montreal: McGill Queen’s University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janis, I. L. (1972). Victims of groupthink. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplowitz, N. (1976). Psychopolitical dimensions of the Middle East conflict. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 20, 279–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H.H. (1979). Personal Relationships. Their Structure and Process. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H., Stahelski, A. J. (1970). Social interaction basis of cooperators’ and competi-tors’ beliefs about others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 66–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelman, H. C. (1982). Creating the conditions for Israeli-Palestinian negotiations. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 26, 39–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W. (1980). Lay epistemologic-process and contents. Psychological Review, 87, 70–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W. (in press). Basic processes in social cognition: A theory of lay epistemology, New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Ajzen, I. (1983). Bias and error in human judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 13, 1–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Freund, T. (1983). The freezing and unfreezing of lay inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping and numerical anchoring. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 448–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Jaffe, Y. (1986). Lay epistemology: a theory for cognitive therapy. In L. Y. Abramson (Ed.), An attributional perspective in clinical psychology. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Klar, Y. (1985). Knowing what to do: On the epistemology of actions. In J. Kuhl J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action control: From cognition to behavior. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luchins, A. S. (1957). Experimental attempts to minimize the impact of first impressions. In C. A. Hovland (Ed.), The order of presentation in persuasion. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. (1941). The frustration-aggression hypothesis. Psychological Review, 98, 337–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing knowledge. In R. H. Winson (Ed.), The psychology of computer vision. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mroz, J. E. (1980). Beyond security: Private perceptions among Arabs and Israelis. New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, V. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in self-perception and social perception: Biased attributional process in the debriefing paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 880–892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, J.Z. (Ed.) (1980). Dynamics of third party intervention: Kissinger in the Middle East. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E., Ortony, A. (1977). The representation of knowledge in memory. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spico, W.E., Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R.C., Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M., Harvey, O.J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., Sherif, C.W. (1961). Intergroup cooperation and competition: The Robbers’ Cave experiment. Norman, OK: University Book Exchange.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. D. (1958). Threat perception and the armament-tension dilemma. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2, 90–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smock, D.C. (1955). The influence of psychological stress on the “intolerance of ambiguity”. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50, 177–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M. L., Wicklund, R. A. (1981). Attribute ambiguity. In J. H. Harvey, W. Ickes, R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 3 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streufert, S., Fromkin, H. (1969). True conflict and complex decision making: The effect of the three party duel and military and economic behavior of decision making groups in complex environments (ONP Technical Report No. 25 ). West Lafayette: Purdue University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suedfeld, P., Tetlock, R E. (1977). Integrative complexity of communications in international crises. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 21, 169–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., Bassok, M. (1983). Information gathering strategies in hypothesis testing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 560–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wall, J.A. (1981). Mediation: an analysis, review and proposed research. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25, 157–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P.C., Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1972). Psychology of reasoning: Structure and content. London: Batsford.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1988 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Klar, Y., Bar-Tal, D., Kruglanski, A.W. (1988). Conflict as a Cognitive Schema: Toward a Social Cognitive Analysis of Conflict and Conflict Termination. In: Stroebe, W., Kruglanski, A.W., Bar-Tal, D., Hewstone, M. (eds) The Social Psychology of Intergroup Conflict. Springer Series in Social Psychology. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-52124-9_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-52124-9_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-52126-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-52124-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics