Skip to main content

Comparison

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Towards a Theory of Thinking

Part of the book series: On Thinking ((ONTHINKING))

Abstract

The process of comparison plays a critical role in problem solving, ­judgment, decision making, categorization, and cognition, broadly construed. In turn, determination of similarities and differences plays a critical role for comparison. In this chapter, we describe important classes of formal models of similarity and comparison: geometric, featural, alignment-based, and transformational. We also consider the question of whether similarity is too flexible to provide a stable ground for cognition, and conversely, whether it is insufficiently flexible to account for the sophistication of cognition. Both similarity assessments and comparison are argued to provide valuable general-purpose cognitive strategies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Allen SW, Brooks LR (1991) Specializing the operation of an explicit rule. J Exp Psychol Gen 120:3-19

    Google Scholar 

  • Attneave F (1950) Dimensions of similarity. Am J Psychol 63:516-556

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bassok M, Medin DL (1997) Birds of a feather flock together: similarity judgments with semantically rich stimuli. J Mem Lang 36:311-336

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein LE, Demorest ME, Eberhardt SP (1994) A computational approach to analyzing sentential speech perception: Phoneme-to-phoneme stimulus/response alignment. J Acoust Soc Am 95:3617-3622

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford JD, Schwartz DL (1999) Rethinking transfer: a simple proposal with multiple implications. Rev Res Educ 24:61-100

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgess C, Lund K (2000) The dynamics of meaning in memory. In: Diettrich E, Markman AB (eds) Cognitive dynamics: conceptual change in humans and machines. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp 117-156

    Google Scholar 

  • Bush RR, Mosteller F (1951) A model for stimulus generalization and discrimination. Psychol Rev 58:413-423

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll JD, Wish M (1974) Models and methods for three-way multidimensional scaling. In Krantz DH, Atkinson RC, Luce RD, Suppes P (eds) Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology, vol 2. Freeman, San Francisco, pp 57-105

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins AM, Quillian MR (1969) Retrieval time from semantic memory. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 8:240-247

    Google Scholar 

  • Doumas LAA, Hummel JE (2005) Approaches to modeling human mental representation: what works, what doesn’t, and why. In Holyoak KJ, Morrison RG (eds) The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, pp 73-91

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisler H, Ekman G (1959) A mechanism of subjective similarity. Acta Psychol 16:1-10

    Google Scholar 

  • Falkenhainer B, Forbus KD, Gentner D (1989) The structure-mapping engine: Algorithm and examples. Artif Intell 41:1-63

    Google Scholar 

  • Frisch SA, Broe MB, Pierrehumbert JB (1995) The role of similarity in phonology: Explaining OCP-Place. In Elenius K, Branderud P (eds) Proceedings of the, 13th International Conference of the Phonetic Sciences, Stockholm, vol 3, pp 544-547

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner WR (1974) The processing of information and structure. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gati I, Tversky A (1984) Weighting common and distinctive features in perceptual and conceptual judgments. Cogn Psychol 16:341-370

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner D (1983) Structure-mapping: a theoretical framework for analogy. Cogn Sci 7:155-170

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner D, Namy L (1999) Comparison in the development of categories. Cogn Dev 14:487-513

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner D, Rattermann MJ (1991) Language and the career of similarity. In: Gelman SA, Byrnes JP (eds) Perspectives on language and thought interrelations in development. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner D, Toupin C (1986) Systematicity and surface similarity in the development of analogy. Cogn Sci 10(3):277-300

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner D, Loewenstein J, Thompson L (2003) Learning and transfer: a general role for analogical encoding. J Educ Psychol 95:393-408

    Google Scholar 

  • Gick ML, Holyoak KJ (1983) Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cogn Psychol 15:1-38

    Google Scholar 

  • Gluck MA (1991) Stimulus generalization and representation in adaptive network models of category learning. Psychol Sci 2:50-55

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone RL (1994a) Similarity, interactive activation, and mapping. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 20:3-28

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone RL (1994b) The role of similarity in categorization: Providing a groundwork. Cognition 52:125-157

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone RL (1996) Alignment-based nonmonotonicities in similarity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 22:988-1001

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone RL, Medin DL (1994) The time course of comparison. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 20:29-50

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone RL, Medin DL, Gentner D (1991) Relations, attributes, and the non-independence of features in similarity judgments. Cogn Psychol 23:222-264

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone RL, Medin DL, Halberstadt J (1997) Similarity in context. Mem Cogn 25:237-255

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths TL, Steyvers M, Tenenbaum JBT (2007) Topics in semantic representation. Psychol Rev 114(2):211-244

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn U (2003) Similarity. In: Nadel L (ed) Encyclopedia of cognitive science. Macmillan, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Hahn U, Bailey RM (2005) What makes words sound similar? Cognition 97:227-267

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn U, Chater N (1998) Understanding similarity: a joint project for psychology, case-based reasoning and law. Artif Intell Rev 12:393-427

    Google Scholar 

  • Hahn U, Chater N, Richardson LB (2003) Similarity as transformation. Cognition 87:1-32

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes-Roth B, Hayes-Roth F (1977) Concept learning and the recognition and classification of exemplars. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 16:321-338

    Google Scholar 

  • Hintzman DL (1986) Schema abstraction in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychol Rev 93:411-428

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstadter D (1997) Fluid concepts and creative analogies: computer models of the fundamental mechanisms of thought. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak KJ (2005) Analogy. In: Holyoak KJ, Morrison RG (eds) The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak KJ, Hummel JE (2000) The proper treatment of symbols in a connectionist architecture. In: Dietrich E, Markman A (eds) Cognitive dynamics: conceptual change in humans and machines. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak KJ, Koh K (1987) Surface and structural similarity in analogical transfer. Mem Cogn 15:332-340

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak KJ, Thagard P (1989) Analogical mapping by constraint satisfaction. Cogn Sci 13:295-355

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak KJ, Thagard P (1995) Mental leaps: analogy in creative thought. MIT, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Horgan DD, Millis K, Neimeyer RA (1989) Cognitive reorganization and the development of chesss expertise. Int J Pers Construct Psychol 2:15-36

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1968). Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J Physiol 195:215-243

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huber J, Payne JW, Puto C (1982) Adding asymmetrically dominated alternatives: violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis. J Consum Res 9:90-98

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummel JE (2000) Where view-based theories break down: the role of structure in shape perception and object recognition. In: Dietrich E, Markman A (eds) Cognitive dynamics: conceptual change in humans and machines. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummel JE (2001) Complementary solutions to the binding problem in vision: implications for shape perception and object recognition. Vis Cogn 8:489-517

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummel JE, Biederman I (1992) Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition. Psychol Rev 99:480-517

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hummel JE, Holyoak KJ (1997) Distributed representations of structure: a theory of analogical access and mapping. Psychol Rev 104:427-466

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummel JE, Holyoak KJ (2003) A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization. Psychol Rev 110:220-263

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imai S (1977) Pattern similarity and cognitive transformations. Acta Psychol 41:433-447

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jakobson R, Fant G, Halle M (1963) Preliminaries to speech analysis : the distinctive features and their correlates. MIT, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • James W (1890/1950) The principles of psychology. Dover, New York (Original work published 1890)

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz JJ, Fodor J (1963) The structure of semantic theory. Language 39:170-210

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotovsky L, Gentner D (1996) Comparison and categorization in the development of relational similarity. Child Dev 67:2797-2822

    Google Scholar 

  • Landauer TK, Dumais ST (1997) A solution to Plato’s problem: the latent semantic analysis theory of the acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychol Rev 104:211-240

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkey LB, Love BC (2003) CAB: connectionist analogy builder. Cogn Sci 27:781-794

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee MD (2002) A simple method for generating additive clustering models with limited complexity. Mach Learn 49:39-58

    Google Scholar 

  • Li M, Vitanyi P (1997) An introduction to Kolmogorov complexity and its applications, 2nd edn. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman AB, Gentner D (1993a) Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cogn Psychol 25:431-467

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman AB, Gentner D (1993b) Splitting the differences: a structural alignment view of similarity. J Mem Lang 32:517-535

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman AB, Gentner D (1996) Commonalities and differences in similarity comparisons. Mem Cogn 24:235-249

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Markman AB, Gentner D (1997) The effects of alignability on memory. Psychol Sci 8:363-367

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman AB, Wisniewski EJ (1997) Similar and different: the differentiation of basic-level categories. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 23:54-70

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin DL, Shaffer MM (1978) A context theory of classification learning. Psychol Rev 85:207-238

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin DL, Goldstone RL, Gentner D (1993) Respects for similarity. Psychol Rev 100:254-278

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell M (1993) Analogy-making as perception: a computer model. MIT, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy GL, Medin DL (1985) The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychol Rev 92:289-316

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Namy LL, Gentner D (2002) Making a silk purse out of two sow’s ears: Young children’s use of comparison in category learning. J Exp Psychol Gen 131:5-15

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Navarro DJ, Griffiths TL (2007) A nonparametric Bayesian method for inferring features from similarity judgments. Adv Neural Inform Process Syst 19:1033-1040

    Google Scholar 

  • Navarro DJ, Lee MD (2004) Common and distinctive features in stimulus representation: A modified version of the contrast model. Psychon Bull Rev 11(6):961-974

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser U (1967) Cognitive psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Nosofsky RM (1984) Choice, similarity, and the context theory of classification. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 10:104-114

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osherson D, Smith EE, Wilkie O, Lopez A, Shafir E (1990) Category-based induction. Psychol Rev 97:185-200

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterholm K, Woods DJ, Le Unes A (1985) Multidimensional scaling of Rorschach inkblots: Relationships with structured self-report. Pers Individ Dif 6:77-82

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer SE (1975) Visual perception and world knowledge. In: Norman DA, Rumelhart DE (eds) Explorations in cognition. Freeman, San Francisco

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmeri TJ (1997) Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 23:324-354

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polk TA, Behensky C, Gonzalez R, Smith EE (2002) Rating the similarity of simple perceptual stimuli: asymmetries induced by manipulating exposure frequency. Cognition 82:B75-B88

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed SK (1972) Pattern recognition and categorization. Cogn Psychol 3:382-407

    Google Scholar 

  • Rips LJ (1989) Similarity, typicality, and categorization. In: Vosniadu S, Ortony A (eds) Similarity, analogy, and thought. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 21-59

    Google Scholar 

  • Rips LJ, Shoben EJ, Smith EE (1973) Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relationships. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12:1-20

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritov I, Gati I, Tversky A (1990) Differential weighting of common and distinctive components. J Exp Psychol Gen 119:30

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross BH (1989) Distinguishing types of superficial similarities: Different effects on the access and use of earlier problems. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 15:456-468

    Google Scholar 

  • Schvaneveldt RW, Durso FT, Goldsmith TE, Breen TJ, Cooke NM, Tucker RG, DeMaio JC (1985) Measuring the structure of expertise. Int J Man-Mach Stud 23:699-728

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanon B (1988) On similarity of features. New Ideas Psychol 6:307-321

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN (1962a) The analysis of proximities: multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function. Part I. Psychometrika 27:125-140

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN (1962b) The analysis of proximities: multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function. Part II. Psychometrika 27:219-246

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN (1972) Psychological representation of speech sounds. In: David EE Jr, Denes PB (eds) Human communication: a unified view. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN (1982) Geometrical approximations to the structure of musical pitch. Psychol Rev 89:305-333

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN (1987) Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237:1317-1323

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN, Arabie P (1979) Additive clustering: representation of similarities as combinations of discrete overlapping properties. Psychol Rev 86:87-123

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin RM, Steyvers M (1997) A model for recognition memory: REM: retrieving effectively from memory. Psychon Bull Rev 4(2):145-166

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon D, Holyoak KJ (2002) Structural dynamics of cognition: From consistency theories to constraint satisfaction. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 6:283-294

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonson I (1989) Choice based on reasons: the case of attraction and compromise effects. J Consum Res 16:158-174

    Google Scholar 

  • Sjoberg L (1972) A cognitive theory of similarity. Goteborg Psychol Rep 2(10)

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman SA (1996) The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychol Bull 119:3-22

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith EE, Sloman SA (1994) Similarity-versus rule-based categorization. Mem Cogn 22:377-386

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith EE, Shoben EJ, Rips LJ (1974) Structure and process in semantic memory: a featural model for semantic decisions. Psychol Rev 81:214-241

    Google Scholar 

  • Tenenbaum JB (1996) Learning the structure of similarity. In: Tesauro G, Touretzky DS, Leen TK (eds) Advances in neural information processing systems, 8. MIT, Cambridge, MA, pp 4-9

    Google Scholar 

  • Tenenbaum JB, De Silva V, Lanford JC (2000) A global geometric framework for nonlinear dimensionality reduction. Science 290:22-23

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgerson WS (1965) Multidimensionsal scaling of similarity. Psychometrika 30:379-393

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman AM (1986) Features and objects in visual processing. Sci Am 255:106-115

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky A (1977) Features of similarity. Psychol Rev 84:327-352

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky A, Gati I (1982) Similarity, separability, and the triangle inequality. Psychol Rev 89:123-154

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky A, Hutchinson JW (1986) Nearest neighbor analysis of psychological spaces. Psychol Rev 93:3-22

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman S (1996) High-level vision: object recognition and visual cognition. MIT, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener-Ehrlich WK, Bart WM, Millward R (1980) An analysis of generative representation systems. J Math Psychol 21(3):219-246

    Google Scholar 

  • Winston PH (1975) Learning structural descriptions from examples. In: Winston PH (ed) The psychology of computer vision. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang S, Markman AB (1998) Overcoming the early entrant advantage: the role of alignable and nonalignable differences. J Market Res 35:413-426

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author Notes

This research was funded by National Science Foundation REESE grant DRL-0910218. Correspondence concerning this chapter should be addressed to rgoldsto@indiana.edu or Robert Goldstone, Psychological and Brain Sciences Department, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana 47405. Further information about the laboratory can be found at http://cognitrn.psych.indiana.edu.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert L. Goldstone .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Goldstone, R.L., Day, S., Son, J.Y. (2010). Comparison. In: Glatzeder, B., Goel, V., Müller, A. (eds) Towards a Theory of Thinking. On Thinking. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-03129-8_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics