Skip to main content

Towards a Neurobiological Understanding of Reduced Self-Awareness During Flow: An Occupational Science Perspective

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Flow Experience

Abstract

Advances in neurobiology, including brain-scanning and physiological measurements, can now be applied to enhance our understanding of reduced self-awareness, one of flow’s core symptoms. It throws light on optimal experience, since self-forgetting links to flow’s positive affect, a paradox in such a highly self-aware species. This chapter synthesises empirical studies into self-awareness, and how it is down-regulated during task performance. A breakthrough was the identification of a specific neuronal network for self-awareness, discovered during brain mapping of goal-directed tasks. The network becomes active whenever tasks end, rather like a default. Evidence suggests that deep engagement in skilled (flow-like) activities dampens the network, relating somewhat to earlier concepts of hypo-frontality. An occupational science perspective proposes that participation in complex activities may reduce the weight of our extreme self-awareness. The human propensity to ‘occupy’ ourselves through a range of activities, such as the arts, can be a major mechanism for stress-reduction. Early twentieth century observations of the restorative function of being occupied transformed into the profession of occupational therapy, arguably originally a kind of ‘flow therapy’.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2011). The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. doi:10.1177/1073858411403316. Neuroscientist 2012 18: 251 originally published online 15 June 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Haung, C., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Evidence for the default network’s role in spontaneous cognition. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104(1), 322–335.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Blakely, R. (2014, October 11). Switch off and book your way to bliss. The Times UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brom, C., Buchtova, M., Sisler, V., Dechterenko, F., Palme, R., & Glenk, L. M. (2014). Flow, social interaction anxiety and salivary cortisol responses in serious games: A quasi-experimental study. Computers & Education, 79, 69–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, F., & Larson, E. A. (1993). Developing and academic discipline: The science of occupation. In H. L. Hopkins & H. D. Smith (Eds.), Willard and Spackman’s occupational therapy (8th ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, F., Wood, W., & Larson, E. A. (1998). Occupational science: Occupational therapy’s legacy for the 21st century. In M. E. Neistadt & E. B. Crepeau (Eds.), Willard and Spackman’s occupational therapy (9th ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper Perenial Modern Classics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). Activity and happiness: Towards a science of occupation. Journal of Occupational Science: Australia, 1(1), 38–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2004). TED talk. https://www.ted.com/talks/mihaly_csikszentmihalyi_on_flow

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.). (1988). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathunde, K. (1992). The measurement of flow in everyday life: Toward a theory of emergent motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 40, 57–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damoiseaux, J. S., & Greicius, M. D. (2009). Greater than the sum of its parts: A review of studies combining structural connectivity and resting state functional connectivity. Brain Stucture and Function, 213, 525–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Manzano, M., Thoerell, T., Harmat, L., & Ullen, F. (2010). The psychophysiology of flow during piano playing. Emotion, 10(3), 301–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Manzano, O., Cervenko, S., Jucaite, A., Hellenas, O., Farde, L., & Ullen, F. (2013). Individual differences in the proneness to have flow experiences are linked to dopamine D2-receptor availability in the striatum. NeuroImage, 67, 1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Di, X., & Biswal, B. B. (2014). Indentifying the default mode network structure using dynamic causal modelling on resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage, 86, 53–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 231–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, A. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of flow. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 746–761.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, A. (2006). Transient hypofrontality as a mechanism for the psychological effects of exercise. Psychiatry Research, 145, 79–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, A., & Audiffren, M. (2011). The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) model of acute exercise. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1305–1325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427(6972), 311–312. doi:10.1038/427311a.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Draganski, B., Gaser, C., & Kempermann, G. (2006). Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(23), 6314–6317. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4628-05.2006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. SCAN, 2, 313–322.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. M. (2008). The prefrontal cortex. Fourth Edition. London: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallwey, W. T. (2015). The inner game of tennis: The ultimate guide to the mental side of peak performance. London: Pan Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentili, C., Ricciardi, E., Gobbini, M. I., Santarelli, M., Haxby, F., Peitrini, J. V., et al. (2009). Beyond Amygdala: Default mode network activity differs between patients with social phobia and healthy controls. Brain Research Bulleton, 79(6), 409–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, I. I., Harel, M., & Malach, R. (2006). When the brain loses its self: Prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron, 50, 329–339. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goleman, D. (2007). Social intelligence: The new science of human relationships. New York: Arrow Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, S. (2008). I.D. -The quest for identity in the 21st century. London: Sceptre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, A. (2013). Unpublished dissertation. University of Brighton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greicius, M. D., Krasnou, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. PNAS, 100(1), 253–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gusnard, D. A., Akdudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raiche, M. E. (2001). Medical prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. PNAS, 98(7), 4259–4264.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, R. (2009). Buddha’s brain. Oakland: New Harpinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmat, L., de Manzano, O., Theorell, T., Hogman, L., Fischer, H., & Ullen, F. (2015). Physiological correlates of the flow experience during computer game playing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97, 1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 363–390.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2013). Making: Anthropology, archeology, art and architecture. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingvar, D. H. (1979). “Hyperfrontal” distribution of the cerbral grey matter flow in resting wakefulness: On the functional anatomy of the resting state. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 60, 12–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ingvar, D. H. (1985). “Memory of the future”: An essay on the temoral organization of conscious awareness. Human Neurobiology, 4(3), 127–136.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonson, S. C., Baxter, J. C., & Wilder, L. S. (2002). Neural correlates of self reflection. Brain, 125, 1814–1818.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klasen, M., Weber, R., Kirchner, T. T., Mathiak, K. A., & Mathiak, K. (2012). Neural contributions to flow experience during video game playing. Social Cognitive Affective Neuroscience, 7(4), 485–495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kringelbach, M. L., & Berridge, K. C. (2010). The functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Discovery Medicine, 9(49), 579–587.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • le Merrer, Becker J., Befort B., & Kieffer B. L. (2009). Reward processing by the opioid system in the brain Physiolog. 2013 Social: Why our brains are wired to connect. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2013). Social: Why our brains are wired to connect. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lou, H. C. (2015). Editorial: Self-awareness – An emerging field in neurology. ACTA Paediatrica, 104, 121–122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovejoy, A. (1933). The great chain of being: A study of the history of an idea. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Northoff, G., & Bermpohl, F. (2004). Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends in Cognitive Neurosciences, 8, 102–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peifer, C. (2012). Psycholophysiological correlates of flow experiences. In S. Engeser (Ed.), Advances in flow research (pp. 139–1650). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M. E., Macleod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Procedures of the National Academy of Science USA, 98(2), 676–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruby, P., & Decet, J. (2004). How would you feel verses how do you think she would feel: A neuroimage strudy of perspective taking with social emotions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(6), 988–999.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Price, J. L., Rundle, M. M., Vaishnavi, S. N., Snyder, A. Z., Mintun, M. A., Wang, S., Coalson, R. S., & Raichie, M. E. (2008). The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. PNAS, 106(6), 1943–1947.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suchoversky, A. (2013). Flow during rock-climbing. Unpublished dissertation, University of Brighton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallis, R. (2005). I am – A philosophical inquiry into first-person being. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uddin, L. Q., Iacoboni, M., Lange, C., & Keenan, P. (2007). The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurones. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(4), 153–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ullen, F., de Manzano, O., Almeida, R., Magnussson, P. K. E., Pedeersen, N. L., Nakamura, J., et al. (2012). Proneness for psychological flow in everyday life: Association with personality and intelligence. Personal Individual Differences, 52, 167–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, M., Keller, J., Hoenig, K., Waller, C., & Gron, G. (2014). Neural correlates of experimentally induced flow experiences. NeuroImage, 86, 194–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, M., Keller, J., & Grön, G. (2016). Neural signatures of experimentally induced flow experiences identified in a standard fMRI block-design with BOLD imaging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosciences. 11(3): 496–507.doi:10.1093/scan/nsv133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Berg, A. E., & Custer, M. H. G. (2011). Gardening promotes neuroendocrine and affective restoration from stress. Journal of Health Psychology, 16(1), 3–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilcock, A. A. (2008). An occupational perspective of health. Thorofare: Slack.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilcock, A. A., & Hocking, C. (2015). An occupational perspective of health (3rd ed.). Thorofare: Slack Incorporated.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2006). Challenge-skills and mindfulness: An exploration of the conundrum of flow process. OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health, 26(1), 25–32. ISSN 1539-4492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerxa, E. J. (1996). Eleanor Clarke Slagle Lecture. Authentic occupational therapy. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 1967XXI(1), 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerxa, E., Clark, F., Jackson, J., Parham, D., Pierce, D., Stein, C., et al. (1989). An introduction to occupational science, a foundation for occupational therapy in the 21st century. Occupational Therapy in Health Care, 6(4), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zemke, R., & Clark, F. (1996). Occupational science: The evolving discipline. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zohar, M., & Marshall, J. (2000). Spiritual intelligence. London: Bloomsbury.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gaynor Sadlo .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sadlo, G. (2016). Towards a Neurobiological Understanding of Reduced Self-Awareness During Flow: An Occupational Science Perspective. In: Harmat, L., Ørsted Andersen, F., Ullén, F., Wright, J., Sadlo, G. (eds) Flow Experience. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28634-1_22

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics