Skip to main content

Cognitive Processes in Time Perspective

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Time Perspective Theory; Review, Research and Application

Abstract

In this chapter we explore the role of cognitive processes in time perspective. Extending previous findings, we present how individual differences in time orientation are related to intelligence, goal-directed behaviour, executive control and meta-cognition. First, we show that present fatalistic TP is negatively related to intelligence, and that future-oriented individuals tend to have higher general abilities. Further, we present the data, proving that risk taking is negatively correlated with the future positive scale. Another finding discussed here is that working memory development is a strong predictor of temporal orientation. Children who have efficient working memory updating functions in early school age exhibit higher tendency toward future positive when they are adolescents. Finally, our studies indicate, that time perspectives tend to correlate with meta-cognitive skills, knowledge and experiences, suggesting that the temporal orientation influences meta-cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ackerman, P., & Heggestad, E. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 219–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Austin, E. J., Deary, I. J., Whiteman, M. C., Fowkes, F. G. R., Pedersen, N., Rabitt, P., Bent, N., & McInnes, L. (2002). Relationships between ability and personality: Does intelligence contribute positively to personal and social adjustment? Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1391–1411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertman, S. (1998). Hyperculture. The human cost of speed. London: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, J. N., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2005). Time perspective, health and risk taking. In A. Strathman & J. Joireman (Eds.), Understanding behavior in the context of time. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantwell, R. H. (1998). The development of beliefs about learning from mid- to late- adolescence. Educational Psychology, 18, 27–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carelli, M. G. (2012). Time perspective on adolescent risk taking and working memory development. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carelli, M. G., Wiberg, B., & Wiberg, M. (2011). Development and construct validation of the Swedish Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (S-ZTPI). European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 27, 220–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 547–552.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Czerniawska, E., & Cantwell, R. H. (2003). Strategic control of learning: A polish replication of the Strategic Flexibility Questionnaire, Kwartalnik Pedagogiczny [Education Quarterly], 1–2, 161–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Bilde, J., Vansteenkiste, M., & Lens, W. (2011). Understanding the association between future time perspective and self-regulated learning through the lens of self-determination theory. Learning and Instruction, 21(3), 332–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dragan, M., & Dragan, W. Ł. (2011). Właściwości psychometryczne polskiej wersji The Metacognitions Questionnaire – 30 [Psychometric properties of the Polish version of The Metacognitions Questionnaire – 30]. Psychiatria Polska [Polish Psychiatry], 4, 545–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dragan, M., Dragan, W. Ł., Kononowicz, T., & Wells, A. (2011). On the relationship between temperament, metacognition and anxiety: Independent and mediated effects. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 25, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Efklides, A. (2006). Metacognition and affect: What can metacognitive experiences tell us about the learning process? Educational Research Review, 1, 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Thomas: Springfield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Figner, B., Weber, E. U., Mackinlay, R. J., & Wilkening, F. (2009). Affective and deliberative processes in risky choice: Age differences in risk taking in the Columbia Card Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 35, 709–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new era of cognitive inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forman, H., Mäntylä, T., & Carelli, M. G. (2011). Time keeping and working memory development in early adolescence: A 4-year follow-up. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 170–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence, 24, 79–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y., Chiu, C., & Dweck, C. S. (1995). Implicit theories of intelligence: Reconsidering the role of confidence in achievement motivation. In M. H. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem (pp. 197–216). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Keough, K. A., Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Who’s smoking, drinking, and using drugs? Time perspective as a predictor of substance use. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 149–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozhevnikov, M. (2007). Cognitive styles in the context of modern psychology: Toward an integrated framework of cognitive style. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 464–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ledzińska, M. (2001). Człowiek współczesny wobec nadprodukcji informacji, czyli o informacyjnym stresie [A contemporary person in an overproduction of information, i.e. information stress]. In W. Ciarkowska, & A. Matczak (Eds.), Różnice indywidualne: wybrane badania inspirowane Regulacyjną Teorią Temperamentu Profesora Jana Strelaua [Individual differences: Selected tests inspired by Professor Jan Strelau’s Regulatory Theory of Temperament]. Warsaw: Interdyscyplinarne Centrum Genetyki Zachowania Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ledzińska, M. (2007). The role of the individual’s intelligence in the area information overflow. Poster presented on the 12th Biennial conference European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, 28 August – 1 September, 2007, Budapest, Hungary.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ledzińska, M. (in preparation). Time perspective and meta-cognition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lennings, C. J., Burns, A. M., & Cooney, G. (1998). Profiles of time perspective and personality: Developmental considerations. The Journal of Psychology Interdisciplinary and Applied, 132, 629–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Deary, I., & Whiteman, A. (2009). Personality traits. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Warm, J. S., Reinerman, L. E., Langheim, L. K., & Saxby, D. J. (2010). Task engagement, attention and executive control. In A. Gruszka, G. Matthews, & B. Szymura (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in cognition: Attention, memory and executive control (pp. 205–230). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, K., & Johnson, M. K. (2000). Source monitoring: Attributing mental experience. In F. I. M. Craik (Ed.), The oxford handbook of memory (pp. 179–195). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R. L. C., & Phillips, L. H. (2007). The psychological, neurochemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 45, 617–629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mongay, J., & Ledzińska, M. (2011). On reducing the detrimental information flood in the use internet. In W. W. Lamanauskas (Ed.), Problems of psychology in the 21st century (pp. 84–95). Siauliai: Scientia Educologica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1983). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 4: Advanced Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinghold, H. (1985). Tool for thought: The people and ideas behind the next computer revolution. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiglitz, E. (2004). Globalizacja [Globalization]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stolarski, M., Bitner, J., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2011). Time perspective, emotional intelligence and discounting of delayed awards. Time and Society, 20, 346–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). Mindful judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 53–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A. (1995). Metacognition and worry: A cognitive model of generalised anxiety disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 301–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A., & Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2004). A short form of the meta-cognitions questionnaire: Properties of the MCQ 30. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 385–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1994). Attention and emotion: A clinical perspective. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajenkowski, M. (2013). Energetic arousal and language: Predictions from the computational theory of quantifiers processing. Human Factors, 55, 924–934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. W., & Howell, R. T. (2011). Do time perspectives predict unique variance in life satisfaction beyond personality traits? Personality and Individual Differences, 50(8), 1261–1266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajenkowski, M., & Stolarski, M. (in preparation). Intelligence and time perspective.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajenkowski, M., Goryńska, E., & Winiewski, M. (2012). Variability of the relationship between personality and mood. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 858–861.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. W., Howell, R. T., & Bowerman, T. (2013a). Validating a brief measure of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory. Time and Society, 22, 391–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. W., Howell, R. T., & Stolarski, M. (2013b). Comparing three methods to measure a balanced time perspective: The relationship between a balanced time perspective and subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 169–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual- differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1271–1288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2008). The time paradox. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G., & Maslach, C. (1992). Cognitive effects of biased time perspective. International Journal of Psychology, 27, 167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G., Keough, K. A., & Boyd, J. N. (1997). Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 1007–1023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marcin Zajenkowski .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Zajenkowski, M., Carelli, M.G., Ledzińska, M. (2015). Cognitive Processes in Time Perspective. In: Stolarski, M., Fieulaine, N., van Beek, W. (eds) Time Perspective Theory; Review, Research and Application. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-07368-2_16

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics