Skip to main content

The Origins of Human Morality: Complex Socio-moral Evaluations by Preverbal Infants

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
New Frontiers in Social Neuroscience

Part of the book series: Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences ((NEUROSCIENCE,volume 21))

Abstract

This chapter reviews a body of recent research suggesting that the capacity to identify and evaluate others based on their prosocial and antisocial acts operates within the first year of life, and is sensitive to many of the same factors that constrain adults’ social and moral judgments, including the role of mental states and context in distinguishing good and bad behavior. The work is offered as empirical support for claims that human capacities for social and moral evaluation are rooted in evolved, reliably-developing systems for distinguishing friend from foe, required for the long-term functioning of cooperative systems.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ambady N, Rosenthal R (1992) Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 111:256

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambady N, Hallahan M, Rosenthal R (1995) On judging and being judged accurately in zero-acquaintance situations. J Pers Soc Psychol 69:518–529

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronson E, Cope V (1968) My enemy’s enemy is my friend. J Pers Soc Psychol 8:8–12

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod R (1984) The evolution of cooperation. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod R, Hamilton WD (1981) The evolution of cooperation. Science 211:1390–1396

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baird JA, Astington JW (2004) The role of mental state understanding in the development of moral cognition and moral action. New Child Adolesc Dev 103:37–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Baird JA, Moses LJ (2001) Do preschoolers appreciate that identical actions may be motivated by different intentions? J Cogn Dev 2:413–448

    Google Scholar 

  • Ballew CC, Todorov A (2007) Predicting political elections from rapid and unreflective face judgments. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:17948–17953

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay P (2006) Reputational benefits for altruistic punishment. Evol Human Behav 27:325–344

    Google Scholar 

  • Behne T, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M (2005) Unwilling versus unable: infants’ understanding of intentional action. Dev Psychol 41:328–337

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair RJR (1995) A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition 57:1–29

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blair IV, Judd CM, Chapleau KM (2004) The influence of Afrocentric facial features in criminal sentencing. Psychol Sci 15:674–679

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1992) Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethol Sociobiol 13:171–195

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandone AC, Wellman HM (2009) You can’t always get what you want. Infants understand failed goal-directed actions. Psychol Sci 20:85–91

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bright SB, Keenan PJ (1995) Judges and the politics of death: deciding between the bill of rights and the next election in capital cases. Bull Rev 75:759

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown DE (1991) Human universals. McGraw-Hill, New York. Retrieved from http://wwwlavoisierfr/livre/noticeasp?id=OOLWOOAS3O6OWA

    Google Scholar 

  • Buresh JS, Woodward AL (2007) Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition 104:287–314

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlston DE, Skowronski JJ (1994) Savings in the relearning of trait information as evidence for spontaneous inference generation. J Pers Soc Psychol 66:840

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler MJ, Sokol BW, Hallett D (2001) Moral responsibility and the interpretive turn: children’s changing conceptions of truth and rightness. In: Malle B, Moses L, Baldwin D (eds) Intentions and intentionality: foundations of social cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 345–365

    Google Scholar 

  • Clutton-Brock TH, Parker GA (1995) Punishment in animal societies. Nature 373:209–216

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides L, Tooby J (1992) Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In: Barkow J, Cosmides L, Tooby J (eds) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo PR, Coie JD, Grumet JF, Farnill D (1973) A reexamination of the effects of intent and consequence on children’s moral judgments. Child Dev 44:154–161

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra G, Gergely G, BĂ­rĂł S, Koos O, Brockbank M (1999) Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition 72:237–267

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra G, BırĂł S, KoĂłs O, Gergely G (2003) One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. Cogn Sci 27:111–133

    Google Scholar 

  • Cushman F (2008) Crime and punishment: distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition 108:353–380

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cushman F, Sheketoff R, Wharton S, Carey S (2013) The development of intent-based moral judgment. Cognition 127:6

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Quervain DJ, Fischbacher U, Treyer V, Schellhammer M, Schnyder U, Buck A, Fehr E (2004) The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science 305:1254–1258

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunfield KA, Kuhlmeier VA (2010) Intention-mediated selective helping in infancy. Psychol Sci 21:523–527

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eberhardt JL, Davies PG, Purdie-Vaughns VJ, Johnson SL (2006) Looking deathworthy: perceived stereotypicality of black defendants predicts capital-sentencing outcomes. Psychol Sci 17:383–386

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farnill D (1974) The effects of social-judgment set on children’s use of intent information. J Pers 42:276–289

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2002) Why social preferences matter–the impact of non-selfish motives on competition, cooperation and incentives. Econ J 112:C1–C33

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Rockenbach B (2004) Human altruism: economic, neural, and evolutionary perspectives. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:784–790

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G, Nádasdy Z, Csibra G, BĂ­rĂł S (1995) Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56:165–193

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert DT, Malone PS (1995) The correspondence bias. Psychol Bull 117:21–38

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2000) Strong reciprocity and human sociality. J Theor Biol 206:169–179

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • GĂĽrerk Ă–, Irlenbusch B, Rockenbach B (2006) The competitive advantage of sanctioning institutions. Science 312:108–111

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK (under review) The eyes have it: the role of cues to agency and goal-directedness in infants’ social evaluations

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK (2013) Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: evidence for an innate moral core. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 22(3):186–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK, Wynn K (2011) Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cogn Dev 26:30–39

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P (2007) Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450:557–559

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK, Hallinan EV, Woodward AL (2008) Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others’ goals. Dev Sci 11:487–494

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK, Newman GE, Wynn K (2009) Eight-month-old infants infer unfulfilled goals, despite ambiguous physical evidence. Infancy 14:579–590

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P (2010) 3-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Dev Sci 13:923–929

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P, Mahajan N (2011) How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:19931–19936

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P (2012) Reply to Scarf et al: nuanced social evaluation: association doesn’t compute. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:E1427

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK, Mahajan N, Liberman Z, Wynn K (2013a) Not like me = bad infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychol Sci 24:589–594

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin JK, Ullman T, Tenenbaum J, Goodman N, Baker C (2013b) The mentalistic basis of core social cognition: experiments in preverbal infants and a computational model. Dev Sci 16:209–226

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauert C, Traulsen A, Brandt H, Nowak MA, Sigmund K (2007) Via freedom to coercion: the emergence of costly punishment. Science 316:1905–1907

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hebble PW (1971) The development of elementary school children’s judgment of intent. Child Dev 42:1203–1215

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heider F (1958) The psychology of interpersonal relations. Psychol Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Helwig CC (2006) The development of personal autonomy throughout cultures. Cogn Dev 21:458–473

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson AM, Woodward AL (2012) Nine-month-old infants generalize object labels, but not object preferences across individuals. Dev Sci 15:641–652

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Henrich N (2007) Why humans cooperate: a cultural and evolutionary explanation. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E, Gintis H, McElreath R (2001) In search of homo economicus: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Am Econ Rev 91:73–78

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, McElreath R, Barr A, Ensminger J, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, Cardenas JC, Gurven M, Gwako E, Henrich N, Lesorogol C, Marlowe F, Tracer D, Ziker J (2006) Costly punishment across human societies. Science 312:1767–1770

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hood BM, Willen JD, Driver J (1998) Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychol Sci 9:131–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy SB (2009) Mothers and others: the evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Imamoglu EO (1975) Children’s awareness and usage of intention cues. Child Dev 46:39–45

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones EE, Davis KE (1965) A theory of correspondent inferences: from acts to dispositions. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 2:219–266

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones EE, Harris VA (1967) The attribution of attitudes. J Exp Soc Psychol 3:1–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley HH (1967) Attribution theory in social psychology. Nebraska symposium on motivation. Retrieved from http://psycnetapaorg/?fa=maindoiL&ing&uid=1968-13540-001

    Google Scholar 

  • Killen M, Smetana JG (eds) (2006) Handbook of moral development. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Killen M, Smetana J (2008) Moral judgment and moral neuroscience: intersections, definitions, issues. Child Dev Perspect 2:1–6

    Google Scholar 

  • Killen M, Mulvey KL, Richardson C, Jampol N, Woodward A (2011) The accidental transgressor: morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition 119:197–215

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhlmeier V, Wynn K, Bloom P (2003) Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychol Sci 14:402–408

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Le DT, Hamlin JK (2013) Ten-month-olds’ evaluations of accidental and intentional actions. Poster presented at the biannual meeting for the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Leach CW, Spears R (2009) Dejection at in-group defeat and schadenfreude toward second-and third-party out-groups. Emotion 9:659

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leon M (1980) Integration of intent and consequence information in children’s moral judgments. In: Wilkening F, Becker J, Trabasso T (eds) The integration of information by children. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 71–112

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie AM, Mallon R, DiCorcia JA (2006) Transgressors, victims, and cry babies: is basic moral judgment spared in autism? Soc Neurosci 1:270–283

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman MD, Gaunt R, Gilbert DT, Trope Y (2002) Reflexion and reflection: a social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 34:199–249

    Google Scholar 

  • Luo Y, Baillargeon R (2005) Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychol Sci 16:601–608

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luo Y, Baillargeon R (2007) Do 125-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? Cognition 105:489–512

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luo Y, Johnson SC (2009) Recognizing the role of perception in action at 6 months. Dev Sci 12:142–149

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malle BF (1999) How people explain behavior: a new theoretical framework. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 3:23–48

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh HL, Stavropoulos J, Nienhuis T, Legerstee M (2010) Six-and 9-month-old infants discriminate between goals despite similar action patterns. Infancy 15:94–106

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauer M (1999) Why are tough on crime policies so popular? Stan L Pol’y Rev 11:9

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonnell PM (1975) The development of visually guided reaching. Percept Psychophys 18:181–185

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN (1995) What infant memory tells us about infantile amnesia: long-term recall and deferred imitation. J Exp Child Psychol 59:497–515

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mikhail J (2007) Universal moral grammar: theory, evidence and the future. Trends Cogn Sci 11:143–152

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak M, Sigmund K (1993) A strategy of win-stay, lose-shift that outperforms tit-for-tat in the prisoner’s dilemma game. Nature 364:56–58

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak MA, Sigmund K (1998) Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature 393:573–577

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak MA, Sigmund K (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature 437:1291–1298

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nucci LP (2001) Education in the moral domain. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Nucci LP, Turiel E (1978) Social interactions and the development of social concepts in preschool children. Child Dev 49:400–407

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Gorman R, Henrich J, Van Vugt M (2009) Constraining free riding in public goods games: designated solitary punishers can sustain human cooperation. Proc Roy Soc B Biol Sci 276:323–329

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget J (1932) The moral judgment of children. Routledge and Kegan-Paul, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack D, Premack AJ (1997) Infants attribute value to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects. J Cogn Neurosci 9:848–856

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ross L (1977) The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: distortions in the attribution process. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 10:173–220

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarf D, Imuta K, Colombo M, Hayne H (2012) Social evaluation or simple association? Simple associations may explain moral reasoning in infants. PLoS One 7:e42698

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smetana JG (1981) Preschool children’s conceptions of moral and social rules. Child Dev 52:1333–1336

    Google Scholar 

  • Smetana JG (2008) Conflicting views of conflict. Mono Sco Res Child Dev 73:161–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Smetana JG, Killen M, Turiel E (1991) Children’s reasoning about interpersonal and moral conflicts. Child Dev 62:629–644

    Google Scholar 

  • Song H, Baillargeon R, Fisher C (2005) Can infants attribute to an agent a disposition to perform a particular action? Cognition 98:B45–B55

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov A, Uleman JS (2002) Spontaneous trait inferences are bound to actors’ faces: evidence from a false recognition paradigm. J Pers Soc Psychol 83:1051–1065

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov A, Mandisodza AN, Goren A, Hall CC (2005) Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science 308:1623–1626

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Turiel E (1983) The development of social knowledge: morality and convention. Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from http://booksgooglecom/books?hl=en&lr=&id=pVwn18pM_x4C&oi=fnd&pg=PR7&dq=Turiel,+1983&ots=KIeblj9Q1l&sig=powE4Z5l7JCk5pwnowyy2_PUK9Q

    Google Scholar 

  • Turiel E (1998) The development of morality. Handbook of child psychology. Retrieved from http://onlinelibrarywileycom/doi/101002/9780470147658chpsy0313/full

    Google Scholar 

  • Uleman JS, Blader S, Todorov A (2005) Implicit impressions. In: Hassin RR, Uleman JS, Bargh JA (eds) The new unconscious. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 362–392

    Google Scholar 

  • Wainryb C (2005) Moral development in culture: diversity, tolerance, and justice. In: Killen M, Smetana J (eds) Handbook of moral development. Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers, Mahwah, pp 211–240

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis J, Todorov A (2006) First impressions making up your mind after a 100 ms exposure to a face. Psychol Sci 17:592–598

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winter L, Uleman JS (1984) When are social judgments made? Evidence for the spontaneousness of trait inferences. J Pers Soc Psychol 47:237

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward AL (1998) Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition 69:134

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodward AL (1999) Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behav Dev 22:145–160

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodward AL (2005) Infants’ understanding of the actions involved in joint attention. In: Eilan N, Hoerl C, McCormack T, Roessler J (eds) Joint attention: communication and other minds. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 110–128

    Google Scholar 

  • Young L, Cushman F, Hauser M, Saxe R (2007) The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:8235–8240

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo PD, Frye D, Rapus T (1996) An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cogn Dev 11:37–63

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Kiley Hamlin .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hamlin, J.K. (2014). The Origins of Human Morality: Complex Socio-moral Evaluations by Preverbal Infants. In: Decety, J., Christen, Y. (eds) New Frontiers in Social Neuroscience. Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences, vol 21. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-02904-7_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics