Skip to main content

The Role(s) of Language in Theory of Mind

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Neural Basis of Mentalizing

Abstract

The chapter reviews four opinions about the relationship between language and theory of mind, focusing mostly on preschool child development. The conduit view assumes language serves the role of expressing concepts formed without the involvement of language, and is compatible with findings that infants have some ability to represent other’s beliefs, and that in brain damage, the ability can be preserved without language. The cultural view holds that language is the means by which the culture conveys a particular theory of minds, and that children develop this understanding through discourse. Much empirical research backs up the idea that children surrounded by such talk develop theory of mind earlier. On the theory that language is a cognitive tool for engaging in complex reasoning, one might explain the slower development in children who have language delays, as well as the finding that adults can be disrupted in false belief reasoning if they do not have simultaneous access to their language faculty, as with dual task studies. Finally, the representational view is considered, in which both children and adults use particular aspects of the grammar of language to represent complex events and reason from them. These complement structures turn out to be among the best predictors of children’s false belief understanding in longitudinal studies, and training studies directed at complement syntax reveal improvement also in children’s false belief reasoning. In further research, innovative methods and controls will be needed to see if any one or more of these theories can provide a decisive account of all of the phenomena.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    I used the more neutral “perceived” because it can happen as easily in Sign as in speech.

References

  • Aksu-Ko, A., & Alici, D. M. (2000). Understanding sources of beliefs and marking of uncertainty: Child’s theory of evidentiality. In E. V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Child Language Conference (pp. 123–130). Stanford, CA: Stanford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aksu-Koc, A., Avci, G., Aydin, C., Sefer, N., & Yasa, Y. (2005, July). The relation between mental verbs and ToM performance: Evidence from Turkish children. Paper presented at IASCL, Berlin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, B.A., de Villiers, J.G. & François, S. (2001) Deficit or difference: African American children’s linguistic paths towards a theory of mind. In M. Almgren, A. Barrena, M-J. Ezeizabarrena, I. Idiazabal, & B. MacWhinney (eds) Research in Child Language Acquisition: Proceedings of the 8th Conference of the International Association for the Study of Child Language. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrés-Roqueta, C., Adrian, J., Clemente, R., & Katsos, N. (2013). Which are the best predictors of theory of mind delay in children with specific language impairment? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48(6), 726–737.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, K. (2005). Chimpanzee theory of mind: Looking in all the wrong places? Mind & Language, 20(5), 521–536.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953–970.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., Carroll, N., Hussain, S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2006). Intact first- and second-order false belief reasoning in a patient with severely impaired grammar. Social Neuroscience, 1(3–4), 334–348.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J., & Baird, J. (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J., & Jenkins, J. (1995). Theory of mind development and social understanding. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 151–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R., Buttelmann, D., & Southgate, V. (2018). Invited commentary: Interpreting failed replications of early false-belief findings: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Cognitive Development, 46, 112–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldo, J. V., Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D., Ludy, C., Raskin, P., & Kim, J. (2005). Is problem solving dependent on language? Brain and Language, 92(3), 240–250.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benn, Y., Zheng, Y., Wilkinson, I. D., Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2012). Language in calculation: A core mechanism? Neuropsychologia, 50(1), 1–10.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brecht, K. F. (2017). A multi-facetted approach to investigating theory of mind in corvids (Doctoral dissertation). University of Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquistion of a theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906–921.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2015). Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: A longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language, and explicit theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 67–78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buttelman, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112, 337–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S., & Moses, L. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2002). The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(6), 657–674.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M. (2013). Language, inhibitory control and false belief reasoning: A longitudinal study (Honors thesis). Smith College.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H., Chen, H., & Yeung, W. (2009). Relations between mental verb and false belief understanding in Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 141–155. https://doi.org/10.1016/jeco.2009.05.004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H., Husan-Chih, C., Creed, N., Ng, L., Wang, S., & Mo, L. (2004). Relative roles of general and complementation language in theory of mind development: Evidence from Cantonese and English. Child Development, 75, 1155–1170. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00731.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, K., & Mitchell, P. (2000). Siblings in the development of executive control and a theory of mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 279–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courtin, C. (2000). The impact of sign language on the cognitive development of deaf children: The case of theories of mind. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 266–276.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courtin, C., & Melot, A. M. (1998). Development of theories of mind in deaf children. In M. Marschark & D. Clark (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness: Volume 2 (pp. 79–102). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, A., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language and family background: Individual differences and inter-relations. Child Development, 70, 853–865.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H. (2006). False belief and language comprehension in Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95(2), 79–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J., & de Villiers, P. (2009). Complements enable representation of the contents of false beliefs: The evolution of a theory of mind. In S. Foster (Ed.), Language acquisition (pp. 169–195). New York, NY: Palgrave McMillian.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J., & Garfield, J. (2017). Evidentiality, questions and the reflection principle in Tibetan: What do children learn when they learn about evidentiality? In F. N. Ketrez, A. C. Küntay, Ş. Özçalışkan, & A. Özyürek (Eds.), Social cognition and environment in language development: Studies in honor of Ayhan Aksu-Koç. John Benjamins series: Trends in language acquisition (pp. 113–130). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J. G. (1999). On acquiring the structural representations for false complements. In B. Hollebrandse (Ed.), New perspectives on language acquisition. Amherst, MA: UMOP.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J. G. (2007). The interface of language and theory of mind. Lingua, 117(11), 1858–1878.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J. G. (2018). Perspectives on truth: The case of language and false belief reasoning. In K. Syrett & S. Arunachalam (Eds.), Semantics in language acquisition, for the John Benjamins series trends in language acquisition (pp. 222–245). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17(1), 1037–1060.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., Lindley, E., Chen, M., & the School Readiness Research Consortium (2015, April). Language, inhibitory control and explicit false belief understanding: A longitudinal structural equation model. Poster presented at SRCD.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, P. A., Burns, F., & Pearson, B. (2003). The role of language in theory of mind development of language-impaired children: Complementing theories. In B. Beachley, A. Brown, & F. Conlin (Eds.), Proceeding of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 1, p. 188). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02072.x

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dahlgren, S., & Trillingsgaard, A. (1996). Theory of mind in non-retarded children with autism and Asperger’s syndrome. A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 463–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1983). Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The “Panglossian paradigm” defended. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6(3), 343–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The acquisition of finite complement clauses in English: A usage based approach to the development of grammatical constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 12, 97–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dungan, J., & Saxe, R. (2012). Matched false-belief performance during verbal and nonverbal interference. Cognitive Science, 36(6), 1148–1156.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding (1st ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., & Brophy, M. (2005). Communication, relationships, and individual differences in children’s understanding of mind. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 50–69). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, 1352–1366.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., & Plomin, R. (1990). Separate lives: Why siblings are so different. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durrleman, S., Burnel, M., De Villiers, J. G., Thommen, E., Yan, R., & Delage, H. (2019). The clinical impact of grammar on mentalizing: A study of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and with Developmental Language Disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durrleman, S., Burnel, M., & Reboul, A. (2017). Theory of mind in SLI revisited: Links with syntax, comparisons with ASD. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52(6), 816–830.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durrleman, S., Burnel, M., Thommen, E., Foudon, N., Sonié, S., Reboul, A., & Fourneret, P. (2016). The language-cognition interface in ASD: Complement sentences and false belief reasoning. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders., 21, 109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durrleman, S., & Franck, J. (2015). Exploring links between language and cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Complement sentences, false belief, and executive functioning. Journal of Communication Disorders, 54, 15–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ebert, S., Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., & Weinert, S. (2017). Links among parents’ mental state language, family socioeconomic status, and preschoolers’ theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, 44, 32–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrant, B. M., Mayberry, M. T., & Fletcher, J. (2012). Language, cognitive flexibility, and explicit false belief understanding: Longitudinal analysis in typical development and specific language impairment. Child Development, 83(1), 223–235. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01681

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farrar, J., Benigno, J., Tompkins, V., & Gage, N. (2017). Are there different pathways to explicit false belief understanding? General language and complementation in typical and atypical children. Cognitive Development, 43(C), 49–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrar, M. J., Johnson, B., Tompkins, V., Easters, M., Zilisi-Medus, A., & Benigno, J. P. (2009). Language and theory of mind in preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42(6), 428–441.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fedorenko, E., & Varley, R. (2016). Language and thought are not the same thing: Evidence from neuroimaging and neurological patients. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1369(1), 132.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Forgeot D’Arc, B., & Ramus, F. (2011). Belief attribution despite verbal interference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 975–990.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gale, E., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Pyers, J. (1996). Language and theory of mind in oral deaf children. In A. Stringfellow, D. Cahama-Amitay, E. Hughes, & A. Zukowski (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 1, pp. 213–224). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hale, C., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science, 6(3), 346–359.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, W. S., Nagy, W. E., & Linn, R. (1984). Spoken words: Effects of situation and social group on oral word usage and frequency. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843–855.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. (2005). Conversation, pretense, and theory of mind. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermer-Vasquez, L., Spelke, E. S., & Katsnelson, A. S. (1999). Sources of flexibility in human cognition: Dual-task studies of space and language. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 3–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinzen, W. (2013). Narrow syntax and the language of thought. Philosophical Psychology, 26(1), 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollebrandse, B., Hobbs, K., de Villiers, J. G. & Roeper, T. (2008) Second order embedding and second order false belief. In A Gavarró & M. João Freitas (Eds.), Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of GALA 2007 (pp. 270–280).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollebrandse, B., & Roeper, T. (2014). Empirical results and formal approaches to recursion in acquisition. In T. Roeper & M. Speas (Eds.), Recursion: Complexity in cognition. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, A. M. (2002). Theory of mind and behavior disorders in children with specific language impairment [Abstract]. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 62(11-B).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Cutting, A. (1999). Nature, nurture, and individual differences in early understanding of mind. Psychological Science, 10(5), 429–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1447.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. (2008). Folk psychological narratives. Cambridge, MA: MIT/Bradford books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knüppel, A., Steensgaard, R., & Jensen de López, K. (2008). Mental state talk by Danish preschool children. Nordlyd: Tromsø University Working Papers on Language & Linguistics, 34(3), 110–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that others will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354, 110–114.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kulke, L., Von Duhn, B., Schneider, D., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Is implicit theory of mind a real and robust phenomenon? Results from a systematic replication study. Psychological Science, 29(6), 888–900.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kyuchukov, H., & de Villiers, J. (2009). Theory of mind and evidentiality in Romani-Bulgarian bilingual children. Psychology of Language and Communication, 13(2), 21–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational deficit. Cognition, 40, 203–218.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, C., Freeman, N. H., Kyriakidou, C., Maridaki-Kassotaki, K., & Berridge, D. M. (1996). Social influences on false belief access: Specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Development, 67, 2930–2947.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Y. (2009). Without language: Adult false belief reasoning with verbal interference. Theses, Dissertations, and Projects. 1469. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.smith.edu/theses/1469

  • Lind, S. E., & Bowler, D. M. (2009). Language and theory of mind in autism spectrum disorder: The relationship between complement syntax and false belief task performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(6), 929–937.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linebarger, M. C., Schwartz, M. F., & Saffran, E. M. (1983). Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics. Cognition, 13(3), 361–392.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74(4), 1130–1144.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., Clancy, J., Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Assel, M., … the School Readiness Consortium. (2015). Impacts of a comprehensive school readiness curriculum for preschool children at risk for educational difficulties. Child Development, 86(6), 1773–1793.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Low, J. (2010). Preschoolers’ implicit and explicit false-belief understanding: Relations with complex syntactical mastery. Child Development, 81(2), 597–615.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Low, J., & Watts, J. (2013). Attributing false beliefs about object identity reveals a signature blind spot in humans’ efficient mind-reading system. Psychological Science, 24(3), 305–311.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B., & Snow, C. (1985). The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language, 12(2), 271–295.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meadow, K., Greenberg, M., Erting, C., & Carmichael, H. (1981). Interactions of deaf mothers and deaf preschool children—Comparisons with 3 other groups of deaf and hearing dyads. American Annals of the Deaf, 126, 454–468.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meinhardt-Injac, B., Daum, M. M., Meinhardt, G., & Persike, M. (2018). The two-systems account of theory of mind: Testing the links to social-perceptual and cognitive abilities. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 25.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Leekam, S., & Rosnay, M. (2013). Mind-mindedness and theory of mind: Mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Development, 84(5), 1777–1790.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C. A. (2001). False belief understanding in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(00)00042-3

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622–646.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mo, S., Su, Y., Sabbagh, M. A., & Jiaming, X. (2014). Sentential complements and false belief understanding in Chinese Mandarin-speaking preschoolers: A training study. Cognitive Development, 29, 50–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (2005). Language pathways into the community of minds. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newton, A. (2006). Verbal interference with adult false belief reasoning (Honors thesis). Cognitive Science, Smith College.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newton, A., & de Villiers, J. G. (2007). Thinking while talking: Adults fail non-verbal false belief reasoning. Psychological Science, 18(7), 574–579.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsson, K., & López, K. (2016). Theory of mind in children with specific language impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Development, 87(1), 143–153.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Papafragou, A., Li, P., Choi, Y., & Han, C. (2007). Evidentiality in language and cognition. Cognition, 103, 253–295.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (2005). Infants’ insight into the mind: How deep? Science, 308, 214–216.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., Ruffman, T., & Leekam, S. R. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65(4), 1228–1238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., Sprung, M., Zauner, P., & Haider, H. (2003). Want that is understood well before say that, think that, and false belief: A test of de Villiers’s linguistic determinism on German-speaking children. Child Development, 74(1), 179–188.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. (2001). Influence of siblings’ perspectives on theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 435–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 459–474.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & Slaughter, V. (2003). Opening windows into the mind: Mothers’ preferences for mental state explanations and children’s theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 18(3), 399–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory-of-mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76(2), 502–517.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli, D. J., & Vonk, J. (2004). We don’t need a microscope to explore the chimpanzee’s mind. Mind and Language, 19, 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyers, J. (2004). The relationship between language and false-belief understanding: Evidence from learners of an emerging sign language in Nicaragua (Ph.D. dissertation). University of California, Berkeley, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyers, J., & Senghas, A. (2009). Language promotes false-belief understanding: Evidence from learners of a new sign language. Psychological Science, 20(7), 805–812.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H. (2012). Do infants have a theory of mind? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 59–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roche, M. (2018). Language and non-verbal cognition in aphasia: Insights from an eye-tracking paradigm (Master’s thesis). Universitat Pompeu-Fabra, Barcelona.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Naito, M., Parkin, L., & Clements, W. (1998). Older but not younger siblings facilitate false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 34, 161–174.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734–751.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, P. A., Hosie, J. A., Gray, C. D., Scott, C., Hunter, N., Banks, J. S., & Macauley, M. C. (1998). The development of theory of mind in deaf children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 39, 903–910.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuel, S., Durdevic, K., Legg, E. W., Lurz, R., & Clayton, N. S. (2019). Is language required to represent others’ mental states? Evidence from beliefs and other representations. Cognitive Science, 43, e12710. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12710

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • San Juan, V., & Astington, J. W. (2012). Bridging the gap between implicit and explicit understanding: How language development promotes the processing and representation of false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 105–122. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02051.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R. (2009). Theory of mind (neural basis). In W. Banks (Ed.), Encyclopedia of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78, 376–396.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2009). Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about object identity at 18 months. Child Development, 80(4), 1172–1196.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Senghas, R. J., Senghas, A., & Pyers, J. E. (2014). The emergence of Nicaraguan sign language: Questions of development, acquisition, and evolution. In Biology and knowledge revisited (pp. 305–324). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M. (1994). Theory of mind and the development of social-linguistic intelligence in early childhood. In C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children’s early understanding of mind: Origins and development. Hilllsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M., Wellman, H. M., & Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of first references to mental state. Cognition, 14, 301–321.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shuliang, M., Yanjie, S., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2014). Sentential complements and false belief understanding in Chinese Mandarin-speaking preschoolers: A training study. Cognitive Development, 29, 50–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2006). Aphasia, language and theory of mind. Social Neuroscience, 1(3–4), 167–174.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. (1963). Behaviorism at fifty. Science, 140(3570), 951–958.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slade, L., & Ruffman, T. (2005). How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory, and false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, V., Peterson, C., & Mackintosh, E. (2007). Mind what mother says: Narrative input and theory of mind in typical children and those on the autism spectrum. Child Development, 78(3), 839–858.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, V., & Peterson, C. C. (2012). How conversational input shapes theory of mind development in infancy and early childhood. In M. Siegal & L. Surian (Eds.), Access to language and cognitive development (pp. 3–22). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Maternal mental state talk and infants’ early gestural communication. Journal of Child Language, 36, 1053–1074.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snedeker, J., & Li, P. (2000). Can the situations in which words occur account for cross-linguistic variation in vocabulary com-position? In J. Tai, & Y. Chang (Eds.), Proceedings of the Seventh International Symposium on Chinese Languages and Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by two-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587–592.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Southgate, V., & Vernetti, A. (2014). Belief-based action prediction in preverbal infants. Cognition, 130(1), 1–10.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrevohn, R., & Howie, P. (1995). Theory of mind in children with autistic disorder: Evidence of developmental progression and the role of verbal ability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 249–263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steel, G., Rose, M., & Eadie, P. (2016). The production of complement clauses in children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(2), 330–341.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). Language and understanding minds: Connections in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 124–149). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2005). Theory of mind, language, and executive functions in autism: A longitudinal perspective. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 239–257). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1994). A second look at second-order belief attribution in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 577–586.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tardif, T., So, C. W.-C., & Kaciroti, N. (2007). Language and false belief: Evidence for general, not specific, effects in Cantonese-speaking preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 43, 318–340.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tardif, T., & Wellman, H. M. (2000). Acquisition of mental state language in Mandarin- and Cantonese-speaking children. Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 25–43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales. Chicago, IL: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, L. (2004). Specific language impairment and theory-of-mind: Is normal language development an essential precursor for on time theory-of-mind development? Unpublished manuscript, University of Western Australia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuller, L., Henry, C., Sizaret, E., & Barthez, M. A. (2012). Specific language impairment at adolescence: Avoiding complexity. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(1), 161–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Hout, A., Harrigan, K., & de Villiers, J. G. (2010). Asymmetries in the acquisition of definite and indefinite noun phrases. In P. Hendriks & C. Koster (Eds.), Special Issue on Asymmetries in Child Language, Lingua, 120(8) 1973–1990.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varley, R., Siegal, M., & Want, S. C. (2001). Severe impairment in grammar does not preclude. Theory of Mind Neurocase, 7(6), 489–493. https://doi.org/10.1093/neucas/7.6.489

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woolfe, T., Want, S. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Signposts to development: Theory of mind in deaf children. Child Development, 73, 768–778.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jill G. de Villiers .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

de Villiers, J.G. (2021). The Role(s) of Language in Theory of Mind. In: Gilead, M., Ochsner, K.N. (eds) The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51890-5_21

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics