Skip to main content

Experimental Design and Analysis in the Laboratory Study of Human Operant Behavior

  • Chapter
Handbook of Research Methods in Human Operant Behavior

Part of the book series: Applied Clinical Psychology ((NSSB))

Abstract

Research methods play an essential role in efforts to describe, understand, and control nature. Consensus about appropriate procedures and practices allows researchers to compare and integrate their observations with those of others. On a deeper level, a researcher’s methods express what is regarded as important in the field under study—and what is not. Consider, for example, the earliest experiments of psychologists in which subjects were asked to report on the contents of their consciousness as they engaged in various activities (Boring, 1950, Chapter 16). This “method of introspection” reflected the conviction that the important questions of psychology pertained to the individual’s mental life. Behavior was of interest, but only insofar as it shed light on consciousness and the like. Later psychologists led by Watson and Skinner came to regard behavior as interesting and important in its own right. This emphasis is reflected in the label attached to the methods originated in Skinner’s seminal work on operant conditioning in rats (Skinner, 1938) and pigeons (Ferster & Skinner 1957): the experimental analysis of behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 229.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Antonitis, J. J., Frey, R. B., and Baron, A. (1964). Group operant behavior: Effects of tape-recorded verbal reinforcers on the bar-pressing behavior of preschool children in a real-life situation. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 105, 311–321.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bachrach, A. J. (1981). Psychological research ( 4th ed. ). New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baer, D. M. (1960). Escape and avoidance response of pre-school children to two schedules of reinforcement withdrawal. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3, 155–159.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, D. M. (1977). “Perhaps it would be better not to know everything.” Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 167–172.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, D. M. (1988). An autocorrelated commentary on the need for a different debate. Behavioral Assessment, 10, 295–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baer, D. M., Wolfe, M. M., and Risley, T. R. (1968). Some current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Journal of the Applied Analysis of Behavior, 1, 91–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakan, D. (1966). The test of significance in psychological research. Psychological Bulletin, 66, 423–437.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, D. H., and Hersen, M. (1984). Single case experimental designs: Strategies for studying behavior change ( 2nd ed. ). New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A. (1985). Measurement scales and the age-complexity hypothesis. Experimental Aging Research, 11, 193–199.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A. (1990). Experimental designs. The Behavior Analyst, 13, 167–171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., and Galizio, M. (1983). Instructional control of human operant behavior. The Psychological Record, 33, 495–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., and Journey, J. W. (1989). Reinforcement of human reaction time: Manual-vocal differences. The Psychological Record, 39, 285–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., and Kaufman, A. (1966). Human free-operant avoidance of “time-out” from monetary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 557–565.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., Menich, S. R., and Perone, M. (1983). Reaction times of younger and older men and temporal contingencies of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 275–287.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., Myerson, J., and Hale, S. (1988). An integrated analysis of the structure and function of behavior: Aging and the cost of divided attention. In G. Davey and C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant conditioning and behavior modification (pp. 139–166 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., and Perone, M. (1982). The place of the human subject in the operant laboratory. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 143–158.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., Perone, M., and Galizio, M. (1991a). Analyzing the reinforcement process at the human level: Can application and behavioristic interpretation replace laboratory research? The Behavior Analyst, 14, 95–105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A., Perone, M., and Galizio, M. (1991b). The experimental analysis of human behavior: Indispensable, ancillary, or irrelevant? The Behavior Analyst, 14, 145–155.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A. and Surdy, T. (1990). Recognition memory in older adults: Adjustment to changing contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 201–212.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1975). Time allocation in human vigilance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 45–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benton, R. G., and Mefferd, R. B., Jr. (1967). Projector slide changing and focusing as operant reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 479–484.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, D. J. (1988). Laboratory lore and research practices in the experimental analysis of human behavior: Designing session logistics-How long, how often, how many? The Behavior Analyst, 11, 51–58.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S. W., and Baer, D. M. (1966). Operant methods in child behavior and development. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant research: Areas of research and application (pp. 718–789 ). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolles, R. C. (1962). The difference between statistical hypotheses and scientific hypotheses. Psychological Reports, 11, 639–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E. G. (1950). A history of experimental psychology(2nd ed.). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, C. M., and Szabadi, R. (1988). Quantitative analysis of human operant behavior. In G. Davey and C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant conditioning and behavior modification (pp. 225–259 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Branch, M. (1991). On the difficulty of studying “basic” behavioral processes in humans. The Behavior Analyst, 14, 107–110.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunswick, E. (1955). Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. Psychological Review, 62, 193–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Busk, R. L. and Marascuilo, L. A. (1988). Autocorrelation in single-subject research: A counterargument to the myth of no autocorrelation. Behavioral Assessment, 10, 229–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buskist, W., and Miller, H. L. (1982). The study of human operant behavior, 1958–1981: A topical bibliography. The Psychological Record, 32, 249–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T., and Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, D. A., Ploog, B., and Fantino, E. (1990). Observing behavior in a computer game. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 185–199.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christie, R. (1951). Experimental naivete and experiential naivete. Psychological Bulletin, 48, 327–339.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M. (1979). How to look at data: A review of John W. Tukey’s Exploratory Data Analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 433–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleveland, W. S. (1985). The elements of graphing data. Monterey, CA: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1994). The earth is round (p.05). American Psychologist, 49, 997–1003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T. D., and Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchfield, T. S., and Perone, M. (1990). Verbal self-reports of delayed matching to sample by humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 321–344.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crosbie, J. (1993). Interrupted time-series analysis with brief single-subject data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 966–974.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darcheville, J. C., Riviere, V., and Wearden, J. H. (1993). Fixed-interval performance and self-control in infants. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 239–254.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dougherty, D. M., Nedelman, M., and Alfred, M. (1993). An analysis and topical bibliography of the last ten years of human operant behavior: From minority to near majority (1982–1992). The Psychological Record, 43, 501–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1953). The structure of human personality. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E., and Case, D. A. (1983) Human observing: Maintained by stimuli correlated with reinforcement but not extinction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 193–210.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B., and Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, T. J., and Lattal, K. A. (1992). Stimulus control of behavioral history. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 5–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gaito, J. (1980). Measurement scales and statistics: Resurgence of an old misconception. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 564–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galizio, M. (1979). Contingency-shaped and rule-governed behavior: Instructional control of human loss avoidance Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 53–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez, F. A., and Waller, M. B. (1974). Handwriting as an operant. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 165–175.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenspoon, J. (1962). Verbal conditioning and clinical psychology. In A. J. Bachrach (Ed.), Experimental foundations of clinical psychology (pp. 510–552 ). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hake, D. F., Vukelich, R., and Kaplan, S. J. (1973). Audit responses: Responses maintained by access to existing self or coactor scores during non-social, parallel work, and cooperation procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 409–423.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, R. J., and Green, G. (1990). Development of conditional and equivalence relations without differential consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 225–237.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (1986). The case of the silent dog-Verbal reports and the analysis of rules: A review of Ericsson and Simon’s Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 351–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., and Brownstein, A. J. (1984). Verbal behavior: Is the human operant lab an ideal place to begin? Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 2, 11–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley. Holland, J. G. (1958). Human vigilance. Science, 128, 61–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., and Lowe, C. F. (1993). Determinants of human performance on concurrent schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 29–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, D. C. (1995). Fundamental statistics for the behavioral sciences ( 3rd ed. ). Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huitema, B. E. (1986). Statistical analysis and single-subject designs: Some misunderstandings. In A. Poling and R. W. Fuqua (Eds.), Research methods in applied behavior analysis: Issues and advances (pp. 209–232 ). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huitema, B. E. (1988). Autocorrelation: 10 years of confusion. Behavioral Assessment, 10, 253–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyten, C., and Chase, P. N. (1991). An analysis of self-editing: Method and preliminary findings In L. J. Hayes and P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior (pp. 67–81 ). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iversen, I. H. (1988). Tactics of graphic design. A review of Tufte’s The Visual Display of Quantitative Information. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 171–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iversen, I. H., and Lattal, K. A. (Eds.). (1991). Techniques in the behavioral and neural sciences: Vol. 6. Experimental analysis of behavior. Amsterdam. Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. M. and Pennypacker, H. S. (1993). Strategies and tactics of behavioral research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A., and Baron, A. (1969). Discrimination of periods of avoidance-extinction by human subjects. Psychonomic Science Monographs Supplements, 3, (Whole No. 37), 53–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1992). Research design in clinical psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Macmillan Co. Keller, F. S., and Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. (1959). Appendix: Suggested discussion topics for contributors of systematic analyses. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 2, pp. 653–673 ). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kratochwill, T. R. (1978). Foundations of time-series research. In T. R. Kratochwill (Ed.), Single subject research. Strategies for evaluating change (pp. 1–100 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kratochwill, T. R., and Levin, J. R. (1992). Single-case research design and analysis: New directions for psychology and education. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, F. (1953). On the statistical treatment of football numbers. American Psychologist, 8, 750–751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. E. (1979). Determinants of human operant behavior. In M. D. Zeiler and P. Harzem (Eds.), Advances in analysis of behaviour: Vol. 1. Reinforcement and the organization of behaviour (pp. 159–192 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. T. (1968). Statistical significance in psychological research. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 151–159.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, F. C., Lalli, J. S., Shea, M. C., Lalli, E. P., West, B. J., Roberts, M., and Nevin, J. A. (1990). The momentum of human behavior in a natural setting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 163–172.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, B. A., Catania, A. C., and Shimoff, E. (1985). Effect of uninstructed verbal behavior on nonverbal responding: Contingency descriptions versus performance descriptions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 155–164.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, B. A., Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., and Sagvolden, T. (1977) Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity to ratio and interval contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 453–467.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matyas, T. A., and Greenwood, K. M. (1990). Visual analysis of single-case time series: Effects of variability, serial dependence, and magnitude of intervention effects. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 341–351.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, J. E. (1994). Learning and Behavior ( 3rd ed. ). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meehl, P. E. (1967). Theory-testing in psychology and physics: A methodological paradox. Philosophy of Science, 34, 103–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1974). Statistical inference for individual organism research: Mixed blessing or curse? Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 647–653.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. K. (1968). Escape from an effortful situation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 619–627.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, J. (1986). Measurement scales and statistics: A clash of paradigms. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 398–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. S. (1979). Statistics: Concepts and controversies. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse, W. H., and Kelleher, R. T. (1977). Determinants of reinforcement and punishment. In W. K. Honig and J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 174–200 ). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A. (1992). An integrative model for the study of behavioral momentum. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 301–316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory ( 2nd ed. ). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, D. C., and Donahoe, J. W. (1991). Shared premises, different conclusions. The Behavior Analyst, 14, 123–127.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parsonson, B. S., and Baer, D. M. (1978). The analysis and presentation of graphic data. In T. R. Kratochwill (Ed.), Single-subject research: Strategies for evaluating change (pp. 101–165 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsonson, B. S., and Baer, D. M. (1986). The graphic analysis of data. In A. Poling and R. W. Fuqua (Eds.), Research methods in applied behavior analysis (pp. 157–186 ). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsonson, B. S., and Baer, D. M. (1992). The visual analysis of data, and current research into the stimuli controlling it In T. R. Kratochwill and J. R. Levin (Eds.), Single-case research design and analysis: New directions for psychology and education (pp. 15–40 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perone, M. (1991). Experimental design in the analysis of free-operant behavior. In I. Iversen and K. A. Lattai (Eds.), Techniques in the behavioral and neural sciences: Vol. 6. Experimental analysis of behavior, Part 1 (pp. 135–171 ). Amsterdam. Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perone, M. (1994). Single-subject designs and developmental psychology. In S. H. Cohen and H. W. Reese (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological contributions (pp. 95–118 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perone, M., and Baron, A. (1980). Reinforcement of human observing behavior by a stimulus correlated with extinction or increased effort. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 239–261.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perone, M., Galizio, M., and Baron, A. (1988). The relevance of animal-based principles in the laboratory study of human operant conditioning. In G. Davey and C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant conditioning and behavior modification (pp. 59–85 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perone, M., and Kaminski, B. J. (1992). Conditioned reinforcement of human observing behavior by descriptive and arbitrary verbal stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 557–575.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poling, A., and Fuqua, R. W. (Eds.). (1986). Research methods in applied behavior analysis: Issues and advances. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfarb, I. S., Newland, M. C., Brannon, S. E., and Howey, D. S. (1992). Effects of self-generated rules on the development of schedule-controlled behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 107–121.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. A. (1985). Speed of behavior and its implications for cognition. In J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging ( 2nd ed., pp. 400–426 ). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W. (1994). Developmental designs revisited. In S. H. Cohen and H. W. Reese (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological contributions (pp. 45–64 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, W. H. (1954). Atlas of men: A guide for somatotyping the adult male at all ages. New York: Harper Bros.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimoff, E. (1984). Post-session questionnaires. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 2, 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipley, C. R., Baron, A., and Kaufman, A. (1972). Effects of time-out from one reinforcer on human behavior maintained by another reinforcer. Psychological Record, 22, 201–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research: Evaluating experimental data in psychology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1969). Generalization gradients and stimulus control in delayed matching-to-sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 745–757.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, S., and Castellan, N. J.,Jr. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences ( 2nd ed. ). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1959). A case history in the scientific method. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 2, pp. 359–370 ). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1966). Operant behavior. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant research: Areas of research and application (pp. 12–32 ). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1984). Coming to terms with private events. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 572–581.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S. S. (1951). Mathematics, measurement, and psychophysics. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 1–49 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tufte, E. R. (1983). The visual display of quantitative information. Cheshire, CN: Graphics Press. Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velleman, P. F., and Wilkinson, L. (1993). Nominal, ordinal, interval, and ratio typologies are misleading. The American Statistician, 47, 65–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wanchisen, B. A. (1990). Forgetting the lessons of history. The Behavior Analyst, 13, 31–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wearden, J. H. (1988). Some neglected problems in the analysis of human operant behavior. In G. Davey and C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant conditioning and behavior modification (pp. 187–224 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, H. (1964). Conditioning history and human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 383–385.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, H. (1969). Controlling human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 349–373.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. C., and Johnston, J. M. (1992). Continuous versus discrete dimensions of reinforcement schedules: An integrative analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 205–228.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wulfert, E., Dougher, M. J., and Greenway, D. E. (1991). Protocol analysis of the correspondence of verbal behavior and equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 489–504.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1998 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Baron, A., Perone, M. (1998). Experimental Design and Analysis in the Laboratory Study of Human Operant Behavior. In: Lattal, K.A., Perone, M. (eds) Handbook of Research Methods in Human Operant Behavior. Applied Clinical Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1947-2_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1947-2_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-1949-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-1947-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics