Skip to main content

Abstract

The manner in which subjects in laboratory research perceive, understand, and respond to the situations created for them has been a topic of widespread interest and great controversy throughout the history of psychology. One could argue, in fact, that no serious examination of human behavior can escape considering the subjects’ understanding of the situations in which they are studied: Regardless of whether people have an accurate understanding of their world, they base their actions (at least in part) on the world as they understand it (see Heider, 1958; Kelley, 1967, 1973; Kelly, 1955; Jones & Davis, 1965). Laboratory research is, of course, an attempt to create a highly controlled, well-specified situation in order to test propositions about subjects’ responses to that situation. This chapter will emphasize that a complete understanding of a subject’s actions in the laboratory requires a careful assessment of the subject’s perceptions of that situation, and also that careful attention be given to a number of potential pitfalls.* Both the essential nature of assessing the subject’s psychological state, and the dangers lurking in such assessment, can be demonstrated by considering as our starting point the role of the manipulation check in experimental research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adair, J. G., & Schachter, B. S. (1972). To cooperate or to look good? The subjects’ and experimenters’ perceptions of each others’ intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 74–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adair, J. G., & Spinner, B. (1981). Subjects’ access to cognitive processes: Demand characteristics and verbal report. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 11, 31–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adair, J. G., Spinner, B., Carlopio, J., & Lindsay, R. (1983). Where is the source of artifact? Subject roles or hypothesis learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1229–1231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anastasi, A. (1976). Psychological testing (4th ed.). New York: Macmillan Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asher, H. B. (1963). Causal modeling (2nd ed.). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Betancourt, H., & Weiner, B. (1982). Attributions for achievement-related events, expectancy, and sentiments: A study of success and failure in Chile and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 13, 362–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bracht, G. H., & Glass, G. V. (1968). The external validity of experiments. American Educational Research Journal, 5, 437–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunson, B. I., & Matthews, K. A. (1981). The Type A coronary-prone behavior pattern and reactions to uncontrollable stress: An analysis of performance strategies, affect, and attributions during failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 906–918.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1963). From description to experimentation: Interpreting trends as quasi-experiments. In C. W. Harris (Ed.), Problems in measuring change. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlopio, J., Adair, J. G., Lindsay, R. C. L., & Spinner, B. (1983). Avoiding artifact in the search for bias: The importance of assessing subjects’ perceptions of the experiment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 693–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsmith, J. M., Ellsworth, P. C., & Aronson, E. (1976). Methods of research in social psychology. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlston, D. E., & Cohen, J. L. (1980). A closer examination of subject roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 857–870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlston, D. E., & Cohen, J. L. (1983). Avoiding bias in the search for artifact: A reply to Carlopio, Adair, Lindsay, and Spinner. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1225–1228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, L. (1977). The negative subject: Myth, reality, or a prior experimental experience effect?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 392–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design & analysis issues for field settings. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crano, W. D., & Brewer, M. B. (1986). Principles and methods of social research. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive: Studies in evaluation dependence. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, A. L. (1957). Techniques of attitude scale construction. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elig, T. W., & Frieze, I. H. (1979). Measuring causal attributions for success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 621–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 215–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fillenbaum, S. (1966). Prior description and subsequent experimental performance: The “faithful” subject. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 532–537.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fillenbaum, S., & Frey, R. (1970). More on the “faithful” behavior of suspicious subjects. Journal of Personality, 38, 43–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaito, J. (1980). Measurement scales and statistics: Resurgence of an old misconception. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 564–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heise, D. R. (1975). Causal analysis. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heisenberg, W. (1958). Physics and philosophy. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshel, R. L. (1980). The purposes of laboratory experimentation and the virtues of deliberate artificiality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 466–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 219–266). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1984). LISREL VI: Analysis of linear structural relationships by the method of maximum likelihood. Mooresville, IN: Scientific Software, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1981a). Estimating the effects of social interventions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1981b). Process analysis: Estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Review, 5, 602–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1980). Research design in clinical psychology. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192–240). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D. A. (1979). Correlation and causality. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidder, L. H., & Judd, C. M. (1986). Research methods in social relations (5th ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krantz, S. E., & Rude, S. (1984). Depressive attributions: Selection of different causes or assignment of dimensional meanings?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 193-203.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W. (1975). The human subject in the psychology experiment: Fact and artifact. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 101–147). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lana, R. E. (1969). Pretest sensitization. In R. Rosenthal & R. Rosnow (Eds.), Artifact in behavioral research (pp. 119–141). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lodge, M. (1981). Magnitude scaling: Quantitative measurement of opinions. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masling, J. (1966). Role-related behavior of the subject and psychologist and its effects upon psychological data. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 14, pp. 67–103). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (1985). Measurement scales and statistics: An examination of construct validity. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 85–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, J. P. (1980). Causal attributions for success and failure: A multivariate investigation of dimensionality, formation, and consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 704–718.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, J. P., & Koelbl, S. L. M. (1982). Students’ test performance: Dimensionality of causal attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 31–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Micheli, J. (1986). Measurement scales and statistics: A clash of paradigms. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 398–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Musante, L., MacDougall, J. M., & Dembroski, T. M. (1984). The Type A behavior pattern and attributions for success and failure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 544–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Bellows, N. (1977). Verbal reports about causal influences on social judgments: Private access versus public theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 613–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17, 776–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orne, M. T. (1973). Communication by the total experimental situation: Why it is important, how it is evaluated, and its significance for the ecological validity of findings. In P. Pliner, L. Krames, & T. Alloway (Eds.), Communication and affect. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostrove, N. (1978). Expectations for success on effort-determined tasks as a function of incentive and performance feedback. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 909–916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 598–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pittman, T. S., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1985). Motivation and attribution: The effects of control deprivation on subsequent information processing. In J. H. Harvey & G. Weary (Eds.), Attribution: Basic issues and applications (pp. 117–141). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodewalt, F. (1984). Self-attribution, self-involvement, and the Type A coronary-prone behavior pattern. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 662–670.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ronis, D. L., Hansen, R. D., & O’Leary, V. E. (1983). Understanding the meaning of achievement attributions: A test of derived locus and stability scores. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 702–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). When dissonance fails: On eliminating evaluation apprehension from attitude measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 28–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1969). The conditions and consequences of evaluation apprehension. In R. Rosenthal & R. Rosnow (Eds.), Artifact in behavioral research (pp. 279–349). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Gaito, J. (1963). The interpretation of levels of significance by psychological researchers. Journal of Psychology, 55, 33–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Gaito, J. (1964). Further evidence for the cliff effect in the interpretation of levels of significance. Psychological Reports, 15, 570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (Eds.). (1969). Artifact in behavioral research. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, D. (1982). The causal dimension scale: A measure of how individuals perceive causes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 1137–1145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinner, B., Adair, J. G., & Barnes, G. E. (1977). A reexamination of the faithful subject role. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 543–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stacy, A. W., Widaman, K. F., Hays, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (1985). Validity of self-reports of alcohol and other drug use: A multitrait-multimethod assessment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 219–232.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S.S. (1946). On the theory of scales of measurement. Science, 103, 677–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S. S. (1968). Measurement, statistics, and the sche-mapiric view. Science, 161, 849–856.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Strube, M. J. (1986, August). Type A behavior and post-performance attributions. Presented at the 94th meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strube, M. J., & Boland, S. M. (1986). Post-performance attributions and task persistence among Type A and B individuals: A clarification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 413–420.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F.G. (1984). Measurement scales and statistics: The misconception misconceived. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 394–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valle, V. A., & Frieze, I. H. (1976). Stability of causal attributions as a mediator in changing expectations for success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 579–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, E. J., Campbell, D. T., Schwartz, R. D., & Sechrest, L. (1966). Unobtrusive measures: Nonreactive research in the social sciences. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, E. J., Campbell, D. T., Schwartz, R. D., Sechrest, L., & Grove, L. (1981). Nonreactive measures in the social sciences. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, S. J., & Cook, T. D. (1972). Subject effects in laboratory research: An examination of subject roles, demand characteristics, and valid inference. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 273–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegener, B. (Ed.). (1982). Social attitudes and psychophysical measurement. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., Nierenberg, R., & Goldstein, M. (1976). Social learning (locus of control) versus attribution (causal stability) interpretations of expectancy of success. Journal of Personality, 44, 52–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, P. (1980). Limitations on verbal reports of internal events: A refutation of Nisbett and Wilson and of Bern. Psychological Review, 87, 105–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D. (1985). Strangers to ourselves: The origins and accuracy of beliefs about one’s own mental states. In J. H. Harvey & G. Weary (Eds.), Attribution: Basic issues and applications (pp. 9–36). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Dunn, D. S., Bybee, J. A., Hyman, D. B., & Rotondo, J. A. (1984). Effects of analyzing reasons on attitude-behavior consistency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Hull, J. G., & Johnson, J. (1981). Awareness and self-perception: Verbal reports on internal states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 53–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, P. T. P., & Weiner, B. (1981). When people ask “why” questions, and the heuristics of attributional search. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 650–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Strube, M.J. (1989). Assessing Subjects’ Construal of the Laboratory Situation. In: Schneiderman, N., Weiss, S.M., Kaufmann, P.G. (eds) Handbook of Research Methods in Cardiovascular Behavioral Medicine. The Springer Series in Behavioral Psychophysiology and Medicine. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0906-0_33

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0906-0_33

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-0908-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-0906-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics