Skip to main content

Control Motivation, Depression, and Counterfactual Thought

  • Chapter
Personal Control in Action

Abstract

The notion that there exists a fundamental need to exert control over or to influence one’s environment has enjoyed a long history in psychology (e.g., DeCharms, 1968; Heider, 1958) and has stimulated considerable theoretical work. Such a need has been characterized by theorists at multiple levels of analysis. Control motivation, for example, has been characterized broadly in terms of proactive (White, 1959) or reactive (e.g., Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Brehm, 1966; Brehm & Brehm, 1981) strivings for control over general or specific (Brehm & Brehm, 1981) and central or peripheral outcomes (Thompson, 1993). Additionally, various types of control strategies used to gain or maintain a sense of personal control have been proposed (e.g., Averill, 1973; Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995; Rothbaum, Weisz, & Snyder, 1982; Thompson, 1981). Modes of control, for instance, have been categorized as either primary or secondary. Primary strategies involve direct action undertaken to produce desirable and avoid undesirable outcomes in the external world, whereas secondary strategies employ primarily cognitive processes undertaken to produce a change within the person. Recently, Heckhausen and Schulz (1995) have further delineated these primary and secondary forms of control according to whether they are based on veridical or illusory causal understandings of the world and whether they are functional or dysfunctional. While most control theorists view primary control as preferable to secondary control, the latter is viewed as critical in the process of adaptation to control failures and in the promotion of future primary control attempts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., Kelly, K. A., Mineka, S., & Clements, C. M. (1990). Comorbidity in anxiety and depressive disorders: A helplessness/hopelessness perspective. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger (Eds.), Comorbidity in anxiety and mood disorders (pp. 499–543 ). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, S. M., & Schwartz, A. H. (1992). Intolerance of ambiguity and depression: A cognitive vulnerability factor linked to hopelessness. Social Cognition, 10, 271–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, S. M., Spielman, L. A., & Bargh, J. A. (1992). Future-event schemas and certainty about the future: Automaticity in depressives’ future-event predictions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 711–723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. A. (1985). Actor and observer attributions for different types of situations: Causal structure effects, individual differences, and the dimensionality of causes. Social Cognition, 3, 323–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. A., & Deuser, W. E. (1993). The primacy of control in causal thinking and attributional style: An attributional functionalism perspective. In G. Weary, F. Gleicher, & K. L. Marsh (Eds.), Control motivation and social cognition (pp. 94–121 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. A., Miller, R. S., Riger, A. L., Dill, J. C., & Sedikides, C. (1994). Behavioral and characterological attributional styles as predictors of depression and loneliness: Review, refinement, and test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 549–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Averill, J. R. (1973). Personal control over aversive stimuli and its relationship to stress. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 286–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1996). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological reactance: A theory of freedom and control. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bulman, R., & Wortman, C. (1977). Attributions of blame and coping in the “real world”: Severe accident victims respond to their lot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 351–363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Principles of self-regulation: Action and emotion. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 3–52 ). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M., & Giannopolous, C. (1993). Dysphoria and decision making: Limited information use for evaluations of multiattribute targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 613–623.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C., Aldwin, C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Depression and coping in stressful episodes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 439–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, C. G., & Lehman, D. R. (1995). Counterfactual thinking and coping with traumatic life events. In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 353–374 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, C. G., Lehman, D. R., Silver, R. C., Wortman, C. B., & Ellard, J. H. (1996). Self-blame following a traumatic event: The role of perceived avoidability. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 557–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, C. G., Lehman, D. R., Wortman, C. B., Silver, R. C., & Thompson, S.C. (1995). The undoing of traumatic life events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 109–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective determinants of behavior. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J. A., & Weary, G. (1993). Depression and the impression formation continuum: Piecemeal processing despite the availability of category information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 636–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J. A., & Weary, G. (1997). The relationships among perceived control, causal uncertainty, and depressive symptomatology: A prospective study. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garber, J., Miller, W. R., & Abramson, L. Y. (1980). On the distinction between anxiety states and depression: Perceived control, certainty, and probability of goal attainment. In J. Garber & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Human helplessness: Theory and applications (pp. 131–171 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garber, J., Miller, W. R., & Seaman, S. F. (1979). Learned helplessness, stress, and the depressive disorders. In R. A. Depue (Ed.), The psychobiology of the depressive disorders: Implications for the effects of stress (pp. 335–364). New York Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1995). The experience of regret: What, when, and why. Psychological Review, 102, 379–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Girotto, V., Legrenzi, P., & Rizzo, A. (1991). Event controllability and counterfactual thinking. Acta Psychologica, 78, 111–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gleicher, F., & Weary, G. (1991). The effect of depression on the quantity and quality of social inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 105–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 53–92 ). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. In W. Stroebe & M. Hew-stone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 141–185). Wiley & Sons. Heckhausen, J. (1991). Motivation and action. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102, 284–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. Hildebrand-Saints, L., & Weary, G. (1989). Depression and social information gathering. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 150–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilton, D. J. (1990). Conversational processes and causal explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 65–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janoff-Bulman, R. (1979). Characterological versus behavioral self-blame: Inquiries into de- pression and rape. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1798–1809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K., & Sherman, S. J. (1990). Constructing and reconstructing the past and future in the present. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 482–526 ). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201–208 ). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating effect of state ver- sus action orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 155–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landman, J. (1987). Regret and elation following action and inaction: Affective responses to positive versus negative outcomes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 524–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer-Verlag. Lipe, M. G. (1991). Counterfactual reasoning as a framework for attribution theories. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 456–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 339–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandel, D. R., & Lehman, D. R. (1996). Counterfactual thinking and ascriptions of cause and preventability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 450–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K. D., & Weary, G. (1996). The influence of chronic control concerns on counterfactual thought. Social Cognition, 14, 292–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K. D., & Weary, G. (1997). Unpublished raw data.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., & McMullen, M. N. (1993). The mental simulation of better and worse possible worlds. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 87–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., & McMullen, M. N. (1995). The impact of perceived control on the imagination of better and worse possible worlds. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 588–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, K. L., & Weary, G. (1989). Depression and attributional complexity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 325–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, K. L., & Weary, G. (1994). Severity of depression and responsiveness to attributional information. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 15–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1989). Toward a motivational and structural theory of ruminative thought. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 306–326 ). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx, E. M., Williams, J. M., & Claridge, G. C. (1992). Depression and social problem solving. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 78–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McMullen, M. N., Markman, K. D., & Gavanski, I. (1995). Living in neither the best nor worst of all possible worlds: Antecedents and consequences of upward and downward counterfactual thinking. In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 133–167 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1986). Counterfactual thinking and victim compensation: A test of norm theory. Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 513–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & McFarland, C. (1989). When a coincidence is suspicious: The role of mental simulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 581–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & McFarland, C. (1990). Counterfactual thinking and social perception: Thinking about what might have been. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 305–331 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E., & Lewis, P. (1977). Recognition memory in elderly patients with depression and de- mentia: A signal detection analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 84–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remedia- tion of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 519–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • N’Gbala, A., & Branscombe, N. R. (1995). Mental simulation and causal attribution: When simulating an event does not effect fault assignment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31, 139–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Tangney, J. P., & Gavanski, I. (1994). “If only I weren’t versus ”If only I hadn’t“: Distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 585–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pietromonaco, P. R., & Markus, H. (1985). The nature of negative thoughts in depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 799–807.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pietromonaco, P. R., & Rook, K. S. (1987). Decision style in depression: The contribution of perceived risks versus benefits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 399–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: Toward an under- standing of inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 272–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pittman, T. S. (1993). Control motivation and attitude change. In G. Weary, F. Gleicher, & K. L. Marsh (Eds.), Control motivation and social cognition (pp. 157–175 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pittman, T. S., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1985). Motivation and attribution: The effects of control deprivation on subsequent information processing. In J. H. Harvey & G. Weary (Eds.), Attribution: Basic issues and applications (pp. 117–141 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 805–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1993). Self-esteem and counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 199–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1995a). Outcome controllability and counterfactual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 620–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1995b). Counterfactual thinking: A critical overview. In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 1–55 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1995c). Functions of counterfactual thinking. In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 169–197 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 5–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanna, L. J. (1997). Self-efficacy and counterfactual thinking: Up a creek with and without a paddle. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 654–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, S. J., & McConnell, A. R. (1995). Dysfunctional implications of counterfactual thinking: When alternatives to reality fail us. In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 199–231 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silver, R. L., Boon, C., & Stones, M. H. (1983). Searching for meaning in misfortune: Making sense of incest. Journal of Social Issues, 39, 81–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweeney, P. D., Anderson, K., & Bailey, S. (1986). Attributional style in depression: A metaanalytic review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 974–991.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tait, R., & Silver, R. C. (1989). Coming to terms with major life events. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 352–382 ). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Pham, L. B. (1996). Mental simulation, motivation, and action. In J. A. Bargh & P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), The psychology of action (pp. 219–235 ). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, S. C. (1981). Will it hurt less if I can control it? A complex answer to a simple question. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 89–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, S. C. (1993). Naturally occurring perceptions of control: A model of bounded flexibility. In G. Weary F., Gleicher, & K. L. Marsh (Eds.), Control motivation and social cognition (pp. 74–93 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, L. W., & McEachren, L. (1983). Psychosocial correlates of depressive symptomatology in adult women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 151–160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., & Edwards, J. A. (1994). Social cognition and clinical psychology: Anxiety, depression, and the processing of social information. In R. S. Wyer, Jr, and T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 289–338 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., & Edwards, J. A. (1996). Causal uncertainty beliefs and related goal structures. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: The interpersonal context (Vol. 3, pp. 148–181 ). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., Elbin, S., & Hill, M. G. (1987). Attributional and social comparison processes in depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 605–610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., & Gannon, K. (1996). Depression, control motivation, and person perception. In J. A. Bargh & P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), The psychology of action (pp. 146–163 ). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., Gleicher, F., & Marsh, K. L. (1993). Control motivation and social cognition. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., Jordan, J. S., & Hill, M. G. (1985). The attributional norm of internality and depressive sensitivity to social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 605–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., Marsh, K. L., Gleicher, F., & Edwards, J. A. (1993). Depression, control motivation, and the processing of information about others. In G. Weary, F Gleicher, & K. L. Marsh (Eds.), Control motivation and social cognition (pp. 255–287 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., Graham, S., & Chandler, C. (1982). Pity, anger, and guilt: An attributional analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 226–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, J. R., Weiss, B., Wasserman, A. A., & Rintoul, B. (1987). Control-related beliefs and depression among clinic-referred children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 58–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Gavanski, I. (1989). Mental simulation of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 161–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wicker, F. W., Payne, G. C., & Morgan, R. D. (1983). Participants descriptions of guilt and shame. Motivation and Emotion, 7, 25–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., Neale, J. M., Stone, A. A., & Rachmiel, T. B. (1990). Self-focused attention, coping responses, and distressed mood in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1027–1036.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yost, J. H., & Weary, G. (1996). Depression and the correspondent inference bias: Evidence for more careful cognitive processing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 192–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1998 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Markman, K.D., Weary, G. (1998). Control Motivation, Depression, and Counterfactual Thought. In: Kofta, M., Weary, G., Sedek, G. (eds) Personal Control in Action. The Springer Series in Social Clinical Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-2901-6_15

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-2901-6_15

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4419-3285-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-2901-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics