Skip to main content

Cognition

Its Origin and Future in Psychology

  • Chapter
Annals of Theoretical Psychology

Abstract

Enterprises such as cognitive psychology, neomentalism, problem-solving research, information-system modeling, and artificial intelligence have gained much prominence since 1950; collectively, they amount to a new psychological school, cognitivism. Each of these enterprises uses the word cognitive in a special (nondictionary) sense, which indicates its positive contributions to the study of so-called cognitive phenomena (perception, attention, memory, and thinking) as well as opposition to (a) the peripheralism and associationism of Hullian behavior theory and (b) the antitheoretical stance of Skin-nerian neobehaviorism. A close and critical examination indicates that, apart from considerable contribution of the cognitivist movement to developing empirical principles and technological applications, there is no substantial or compelling evidence that cognitivistic constructs have advanced our theoretical understanding of psychological phenomena. Further, by remaining parochial, cognitivists have failed to promote the formulation of a comprehensive framework that would integrate their work with the main body of established knowledge in the fields of learning, motivation, response production, and neuropsychological processes. There is, thus, little reason to believe that cognitive psychology or cognitive science will incorporate or become the psychology of the future, or will continue its ascendancy. Behind the recent bandwagon excitement of cognitivism, work toward a general psychological theory, incorporating gains made by all psychological enterprises, has continued, and this will probably remain the most promising approach to understanding mind.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport, F. H. Social psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: Houghton Mifflin, 1924.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Henry Holt, 1937.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A. R. Minds and machines. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. A. A simple neural network generating an interactive memory. Mathematical Biosciences, 1972, 14, 197–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A. The metaphorical brain. New York: Wiley-Interscience, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bain, A. The senses and the intellect (3rd ed.). London: Longmans, Green, 1868.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, J. C. Studies of the cognitive representation of spatial relations: I. Overview. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1979, 108, 90–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Behavior theory and models of man. American Psychologist, 1974, 29, 859–869.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. Thinking: An experimental and social study. New York: Basic Books, 1958.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. Structure and direction in thinking. New York: Wiley, 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E. G. A history of experimental psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E. G. A history of introspection. Psychological Bulletin, 1953, 50, 169–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. Imagery as a relational organizer in associative learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1970, 9, 529–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. Judgments and explanations as criteria for the presence of cognitive structures. Psychological Bulletin, 1973, 79, 172–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G., & Nagle, M. Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1979, 5, 692–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bridges, K. M. B. Emotional development in early infancy. Child Development, 1932, 3, 324–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. Perception and communication. London: Pergamon Press, 1958.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. Decision and stress. London: Academic Press, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. In defence of empirical psychology. London: Methuen, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. Words and things. New York: Free Press, 1958.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. Toward a theory of instruction. New York: Norton, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brimer, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. A study of thinking. New York: Wiley, 1956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. Perception and understanding in young children: An experimental approach. London: Methuen, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunge, M. The mind-body problem: A psychobiological approach. New York: Pergamon, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bush, R. R., & Mosteller, F. Stochastic models for learning. New York: Wiley, 1955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. Review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal behavior. Language. 1959, 35, 26–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. Language and mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L. L., & Shepard, R. N. Chronometrie studies of the rotation of mental images. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, M. Intelligence: Its organization and development. New York: Academic Press, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. Personality and psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, V. I. Stop, look and listen: The problem of sustained attention and impulse control in hyperactive and normal children. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 1972, 4, 259–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncker, K. On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 1945, 58 (Whole No. 270).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M., & Switzer, G. Cognitive aspects of information processing: 1. The familiarity of S-R sets and subsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 63, 321–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Princeton, N. J.: Van Nostrand, 1963.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. New introductory lectures of psychoanalysis (trans, by W. J. H. Sprott). New York: Norton, 1933.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, G. G. Self-awareness in primates. American Scientist, 1979, 67, 417–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. The social construction of self-knowledge. In T. Mischel (Ed.), The self: Psychological and philosophical issues. Oxford: Blackwell, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. The perception of the visual world. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, R. (Ed.). Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K. The organism: A holistic approach to biology derived from pathological data in man. New York: American Book, 1939.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. Psychology of learning 1960–80: One participant’s observations. American Psychologist, 1980, 35, 713–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of learning. New York: Harper & Row, 1935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 1949, 56, 51–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harnad, S. Neoconstructivism: A unifying theme for the cognitive sciences. In T. W. Simon & R. J. Scholes (Eds.), Language, mind and brain: An interdisciplinary perspective. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haugeland, J. The nature and plausibility of cognitivism. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1978, 1, 215–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Head, H. Studies in neurology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1920.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. Organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1949.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. The semi-autonomous process, its nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 1963, 18, 16–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. Concerning imagery. Psychological Review, 1968, 75, 466–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. What psychology is about. American Psychologist, 1974, 29, 71–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. Essay on mind. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidbreder, E. Seven psychologies. New York: Appleton-Century, 1933.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R., & Marquis, D. G. Conditioning and learning. New York: Appleton-Century, 1940.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holt, E. B. The Freudian wish and its place in ethics. New York: Holt, 1915.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Honig, W. K. On the conceptual nature of cognitive terms: An initial essay. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century, 1943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, G. Thinking. New York: Wiley, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, W. S. The sensory control of the maze habit in the white rat. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1929, 36, 505–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. The principles of psychology. New York: Holt, 1890.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, F. S. The definition of psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1937.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. Language communities, search cells, and the psychological studies. NebraskaSymposium on Motivation 1975. Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. Psychology and the future. American Psychologist, 1978, 33, 631–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. Integrative activity of the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krechevsky, I. “Hypotheses” in rats. Psychological Review, 1932, 39, 516–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. The essential tension. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Külpe, O. Outlines of psychology (trans, by E. B. Titchener). London: Swan Sonnenschein, 1901. [Originally published, 1893.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J., McDermott, J., Simon, D. P., & Simon, H. A. Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems. Science, 1980, 208, 1335–1342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. Persistent problems in the evolution of mind. Quarterly Review of Biology, 1949, 24, 28–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. The problem of serial order in behaviour. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behaviour. New York: Wiley, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leahy, R. L., & Huard, C. Role taking and self-image disparity in children. Developmental Psychology, 1976, 12, 504–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacCorquodale, K., & Meehl, P. E. On a distinction between hypothetical constructs and intervening variables. Psychological Review, 1948, 55, 95–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, M. J. Reflections on the cognitive-learning trend in psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 1977, 32, 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. From association to structure. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 415–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. H. A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 1943, 50, 370–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, W. Environmental models and mental modes: Representational processes in the great apes and man. American Psychologist, 1976, 31, 284–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDougall, W. An introduction to social psychology. London: Methuen, 1908.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Meichenbaum, D. Cognitive-behavior modification. New York: Plenum Press, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melton, A. W. (Ed.). Categories of human learning. New York: Academic Press, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michotte, A. La perception de la causalité. Louvain: Institut Supérieur de Philosophie, 1946.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. Language and communication. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1951.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. What is information measurement? American Psychologist, 1953, 8, 3–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1960.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. Social learning and imitation. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1941.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. On the interface of cognition and personality: Beyond the person-situation debate. American Psychologist, 1979, 34, 740–754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, C. L. Introduction to comparative psychology (2nd ed.). London: Scott, 1903.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. Preparatory set (expectancy)—A determinant in motivation and learning. Psychological Review, 1938, 45, 61–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and personality dynamics. New York: Ronald, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. Historical introduction in modern psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace, 1949.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, H. A. Explorations in personality: A clinical and experimental study of fifty men of college age. New York: Oxford University Press, 1938.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. Cognition and reality. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. You can’t play 20 questions with nature and win: Projective comments on the papers of this symposium. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, H. W. Analysis of complex conditional reaction in chimpanzee. Journal of Comparative and Psychological Psychology, 1951, 44, 9–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (Ed.). Models of human memory. New York: Academic Press, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. Neomentalism. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1975, 29, 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. The psychology of intelligence (trans, by M. Piercy & D. E. Berlyne). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1947.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. Understanding causality. New York: Norton, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poincaré, H. Science and method (trans, by F. Maitland). New York: Dover, 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. Behavior: The control of perception. Chicago: Aldine, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D. Intelligence in ape and man. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. Computational models and empirical constraints. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1978, 1, 93–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Requin, J. (Ed.) Attention and performance VII. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. N. An intellectual history of psychology. New York: Macmillan, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. Animal intelligence. New York: D. Appleton, 1883.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozeboom, W. W. Mediation variables in scientific theory. Psychological Review, 1956, 63, 249–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. What is a schema anyway? Contemporary Psychology, 1980, 25, 814–816.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C, & Abelson, R. P. Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. Minds, brains, and programs. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1980, 3, 417–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R. Survey of objective studies of psychoanalytic concepts. New York: Social Science Research Council, 1943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. E. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 1948, 27, 379–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. The mental image. American Psychologist, 1978, 33, 125–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Behaviorism at fifty. Science, 1963, 140, 951–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sommerhoff, G. Logic of the living brain. New York: Wiley, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. The principles of psychology (2 vols.). New York: D. Appleton, 1872–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. Component processes in analogical reasoning. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 353–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stout, G. F. Analytic psychology. New York: Macmillan, 1896.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. G. The behavioral basis of perception. New Haven: Yale University press, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. Animal intelligence. New York: Macmillan, 1911.

    Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and attention. New York: Macmillan, 1908.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. Experimental psychology of the thought-process. New York: Macmillan, 1909.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Century, 1932.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 1948, 55, 189–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, S. Against direct perception. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1980, 3, 373–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uttal, W. R. The psychobiology of mind. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. Psychological principles.Cambridge, Mass.: University Press, 1918.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist. Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1919.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. Productive thinking. New York: Harper, 1945.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, N. Cybernetics. New York: Wiley, 1948.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, K. V. From associations to structure: The course of cognition. New York: North-Holland, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S. Contemporary schools of psychology. New York: Ronald, 1931/48.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R. M. Animal psychology and the criteria of the psychic. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, 1905, 2, 141–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziller, R. C. The social self. New York: Pergamon Press, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziman, J. Reliable knowledge: An exploration of the grounds for belief in science. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1984 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bindra, D. (1984). Cognition. In: Royce, J.R., Mos, L.P. (eds) Annals of Theoretical Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-6450-8_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-6450-8_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4615-6452-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-6450-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics