Skip to main content

Auditory Imagery Contains More Than Audition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Multisensory Imagery

Abstract

Contributions of nonauditory information to auditory imagery are examined. The spontaneous appearance of visual imagery concurrent with intentionally formed auditory imagery, and the similarities of spatial-temporal properties, mnemonic properties, and perceptual properties of auditory imagery and of visual imagery, is considered. A hypothesized distinction between an “inner voice” (which contains kinesthetic information related to speech articulation) and an “inner ear” (which does not) in auditory imagery is discussed, and evidence consistent (verbal transformation effect, judgments and comparisons of imaged content, clinical studies) and inconsistent (evidence against existence of a separate phonological loop, pre-articulatory auditory verbal imagery) with this distinction is considered. Possible relationships of auditory imagery to kinesthetic information from practice and performance of music and dance are considered, and the relationship of auditory imagery and synesthesia is briefly considered.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 229.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Godøy (2001) argued “sound and sound source are inseparable in most cases of music cognition” (p. 242), but it could be suggested that his argument applies to auditory cognition more generally, especially given Bregman’s (1990) suggestion that hearing evolved to inform listeners about stimuli in the environment. Visual or kinesthetic imagery of a sound source or how a sound was produced might aid in anticipating subsequent actions of or toward the sound source, and this would be consistent with theories that perception is influenced by possible action (e.g., Hommel et al. 2001; Proffitt 2006). On a related note, Baker (2001) argued that kinesthetic and visual imagery of the keyboard influenced composition of music during the past several centuries.

  2. 2.

    As noted by Zatorre and Halpern (2005) and Hubbard (2010), some researchers suggest or claim a role for auditory imagery in accounting for a specific experimental outcome even if there was no evidence that imagery was actually generated and used in the experimental task. For the sake of completeness, studies that suggest a role for imagery are included in the current chapter, but it is noted if studies do not provide sufficient evidence auditory imagery was actually generated and used in the experimental task.

  3. 3.

    The relationship between emotion and imagery is considered in Chap. 19 in this volume, and so just a few points specific to auditory imagery are mentioned here. First, music is a promising venue for exploring the relationship of auditory imagery and emotion (for a review of music and emotion, see Juslin and Sloboda 2001; also Collier and Hubbard 2001; Gagnon and Peretz 2003; Schubert 2004). Second, emotional experience of music might be related to the ability of music to evoke spontaneous visual imagery (Juslin and Västfjäll 2008), and this has implications for multisensory and crossmodal imagery.

References

  • Abramson M, Goldinger SD (1997) What the reader’s eye tells the mind’s ear: silent reading activates inner speech. Percept Psychophys 59:1059–1068

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Aleman A, Formisano E, Koppenhagen H, Hagoort P, de Hann EHF, Kahn RS (2005) The functional neuroanatomy of metrical stress evaluation of perceived and imaged spoken words. Cereb Cortex 15:221–228

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aleman A, van’t Wout M (2004) Subvocalization in auditory-verbal imagery: just a form of motor imagery? Cognitive Processes 5:228–231

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander JD, Nygaard LC (2008) Reading voices and hearing text: talker-specific auditory imagery in reading. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 34:446–459

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amedi A, Malach R, Pascual-Leone A (2005) Negative BOLD differentiates visual imagery and perception. Neuron 48:859–872

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Aziz-Zadeh L, Cattaneo L, Rochat M, Rizzolatti G (2005) Covert speech arrest induced by rTMS over both motor and nonmotor left hemisphere frontal site. J Cogn Neurosci 17:928–938

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Azulay H, Striem E, Amedi A (2009) Negative BOLD in sensory cortices during verbal memory: a component in generating internal representations? Brain Topogr 21:221–231

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD (1986) Working Memory. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD (2000) The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci 4:417–423

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD, Lewis VJ, Vallar G (1984) Exploring the articulatory loop. Q J Exp Psychol 36:233–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD, Logie RH (1992) Auditory imagery and working memory. In: Reisberg D (ed) Auditory Imagery. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker JM (2001) The keyboard as a basis for imagery of pitch relations. In: Godøy RI, Jørgensen H (eds) Musical Imagery. Taylor & Francis, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S, Harrison JE (eds) (1997) Synaesthesia: Classic and Contemporary Readings. MIT Press/Blackwell, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (2008) Grounded cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 59:617–645

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behne KE, Wöllner C (2011) Seeing or hearing the pianists? A synopsis of an early audiovisual perception experiment and a replication. Music Sci 15:324–342

    Google Scholar 

  • Berz WL (1995) Working memory in music: a theoretical model. Music Percept 12:353–364

    Google Scholar 

  • Bläsing B, Puttke M, Schack T (eds) (2010) The Neurocognition of Dance: Mind, Movement, and Motor Skills. Taylor & Francis/Psychology Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower GH (1970) Analysis of a mnemonic device. Am Sci 58:496–510

    Google Scholar 

  • Bregman AS (1990) Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Brodsky W, Henik A, Rubenstein BS, Zorman M (2003) Auditory imagery from musical notation in expert musicians. Percept Psychophys 65:602–612

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brodsky W, Kessler Y, Rubenstein BS, Ginsborg J, Henik A (2008) The mental representation of music notation: notational audiation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 34:427–445

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bunzeck N, Wuestenberg T, Lutz K, Heinze HJ, Jancke L (2005) Scanning silence: mental imagery of complex sounds. Neuroimage 26:1119–1127

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cahn D (2008) The effects of varying ratios of physical and mental practice, and task difficulty on performance of a tonal pattern. Psychol Music 36:179–191

    Google Scholar 

  • Callan DE, Tsytsarev V, Hanakawa T, Callan AM, Katsuhara M, Fukuyama H, Turner R (2006) Song and speech: brain regions involved with perception and covert production. Neuroimage 31:1327–1342

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chambers D, Reisberg D (1985) Can mental images be ambiguous? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 11:317–328

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen-Kadosh R, Sagiv N, Linden DEJ, Robertson LC, Elinger G, Henik A (2005) When blue is larger than red: colors influence numerical cognition in synesthesia. J Cogn Neurosci 17:1766–1773

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collier WG, Hubbard TL (2001) Musical scales and evaluations of happiness and awkwardness: effects of pitch, direction, and scale mode. Am J Psychol 114:355–375

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Costa A, Roelstraete B, Hartsuiker RJ (2006) The lexical bias effect in bilingual speech production: evidence for feedback between lexical and sublexical levels across languages. Psychon Bull Rev 13:972–977

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Craver-Lemley C, Reeves A (1992) How visual imagery interferes with vision. Psychol Rev 99:633–649

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder RG (1989) Imagery for musical timbre. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 15:472–478

    Google Scholar 

  • Cupchik GC, Phillips K, Hill DS (2001) Shared processes in spatial rotation and musical permutation. Brain Cogn 46:373–382

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cytowic RE (1989) Synesthesia: A Union of the Senses. Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Daselaar SM, Porat Y, Huijbers W, Pennartz CMA (2010) Modality-specific and modality-independent components of the human imagery system. Neuroimage 52:677–685

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dogantan-Dack M (2006) The body behind music: precedents and prospects. Psychol Music 34:449–464

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas KM, Bilkey DK (2007) Amusia is associated with deficits in spatial processing. Nat Neurosci 10:915–921

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch D (1987) The tritone paradox: effects of spectral variables. Percept Psychophys 41:563–575

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eardley AF, Pring L (2006) Remembering the past and imagining the future: a role for nonvisual imagery in the everyday cognition of blind and sighted people. Memory 14:925–936

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eitan Z, Granot RY (2006) How music moves: musical parameters and listeners’ images of motion. Music Percept 23:221–247

    Google Scholar 

  • Elkin J, Leuthold H (2011) The representation of pitch in auditory imagery: evidence from S-R compatibility and distance effects. Eur J Cogn Psychol 23:76–91

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans CL, McGuire PK, David AS (2000) Is auditory imagery defective in patients with auditory hallucinations? Psychol Med 30:137–148

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Farah MJ (1985) Psychophysical evidence for a shared representational medium for mental images and percepts. J Exp Psychol Gen 114:93–103

    Google Scholar 

  • Finke RA (1980) Levels of equivalence in imagery and perception. Psychol Rev 87:113–132

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Finke RA (1985) Theories relating mental imagery to perception. Psychol Bull 98:236–259

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Finke RA (1986) Some consequences of visualization in pattern identification and detection. Am J Psychol 99:257–274

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse P (1982) Rhythm and tempo. In: Deutsch D (ed) The Psychology of Music, 1st edn. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Friberg A, Sundberg J (1999) Does music performance allude to locomotion? A model of final ritardandi derived from measurements of stopping runners. J Acoust Soc Am 105:1469–1484

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagnon L, Peretz I (2003) Mode and tempo relative contributions to “happy-sad” judgments in equitone melodies. Cogn Emot 17:25–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD (1993) Working Memory and Language. Erlbaum, Hove, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs RW (2006) Embodiment and Cognitive Science. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Godøy RI (2001) Imagined action, excitation, and resonance. In: Godøy RI, Jørgensen H (eds) Musical Imagery. Taylor & Francis, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern AR (1988a) Mental scanning in auditory imagery for songs. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 14:434–443

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern AR (1988b) Perceived and imaged tempos of familiar songs. Music Percept 6:193–202

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern AR (1989) Memory for the absolute pitch of familiar songs. Mem Cognit 17:572–581

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern AR, Zatorre RJ (1999) When that tune runs through your head: a PET investigation of auditory imagery for familiar melodies. Cereb Cortex 9:697–704

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern AR, Zatorre RJ, Bouffard M, Johnson JA (2004) Behavioral and neural correlates of perceived and imagined musical timbre. Neuropsychologia 42:1281–1292

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heinen JRK, Cobb L, Pollard JW (1976) Word imagery modalities and learning in the deaf and hearing. J Psychol 93:191–195

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Highben Z, Palmer C (2004) Effects of auditory and motor mental practice in memorized piano performance. Bull Counc Res Music Educ 159:58–65

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel B, Müsseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849–937

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard TL (2010) Auditory imagery: empirical findings. Psychol Bull 136:302–329

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard TL, Ruppel SE (in press) A Fröhlich effect and representational gravity in memory for auditory pitch. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard TL, Stoeckig K (1988) Musical imagery: generation of tones and chords. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 14:656–667

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Huijbers W, Pennartz CMA, Rubin DC, Daselaar SM (2011) Imagery and retrieval of auditory and visual information: neural correlates of successful and unsuccessful performance. Neuropsychologia 49:1730–1740

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Intons-Peterson MJ (1980) The role of loudness in auditory imagery. Mem Cognit 8:385–393

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Intons-Peterson MJ (1992) Components of auditory imagery. In: Reisberg D (ed) Auditory Imagery. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Johns LC, Rossell S, Frith C, Ahmad F, Hemsley D, Kuipers E, McGuire PK (2001) Verbal self-monitoring and auditory verbal hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia. Psychol Med 31:705–715

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones DM (1993) Objects, streams and threads of auditory attention. In: Baddeley AD, Weiskrantz L (eds) Attention: Selection, Awareness and Control. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones DM, Macken WJ (1993) Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect: implications for phonological coding in working memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 19:369–381

    Google Scholar 

  • Juchniewicz J (2008) The influence of physical movement on the perception of musical performance. Psychol Music 36:417–427

    Google Scholar 

  • Juslin PN, Sloboda JA (eds) (2001) Music and Emotion: Theory and Research. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Juslin PN, Västfjäll D (2008) Emotional responses to music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behav Brain Sci 31:559–621

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kadosh RC, Kadosh KC, Henik A (2007) The neuronal correlate of bidirectional synesthesia: a combined event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Cogn Neurosci 19:2050–2059

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller PE, Appel M (2010) Individual differences, auditory imagery, and the coordination of body movements and sounds in musical ensembles. Music Percep 28:27–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller TA, Cowan N, Saults JS (1995) Can auditory memory for tone pitch be rehearsed? J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 21:635–645

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Keller PE, Dalla Bella S, Koch I (2010) Auditory imagery shapes movement timing and kinematics: evidence from a musical task. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36:508–513

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller PE, Koch I (2006) The planning and execution of short auditory sequences. Psychon Bull Rev 13:711–716

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller PE, Koch I (2008) Action planning in sequential skills: relations to music performance. Q J Exp Psychol 61:275–291

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich G, Sebanz N (2006) The social nature of perception and action. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 15:99–104

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM (1980) Image and Mind. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM (1994) Image and Brain. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM, Ganis G, Thompson WL (2001) Neural foundations of imagery. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:635–642

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM, Pascual-Leone A, Felician O, Camposano S, Keenan JP, Thompson WL, Ganis G, Sukel KE, Alpert NM (1999) The role of area 17 in visual imagery: convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science 284:167–170

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM, Seger C, Pani JR, Hillger LA (1990) When is imagery used in everyday life? A diary study. J Ment Imagery 14:131–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Krasnow DH, Chatfield SJ, Barr S, Jensen JL, Dufek JS (1997) Imagery and conditioning practices for dancers. Dance Res J 29:43–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroll NE, Schepeler EM, Angin KT (1986) Bizarre imagery: the misremembered mnemonic. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 12:42–53

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhansl CL, Schenck DL (1997) Can dance reflect the structural and expressive qualities of music? A perceptual experiment on Balachine’s choreography of Mozart’s Divertimento No. 15. Musicae Scientiae 1:63–85

    Google Scholar 

  • Langheim FJP, Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Duyn JH, Weinberger DR (2002) Cortical systems associated with covert music rehearsal. Neuroimage 16:901–908

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lim S, Lippman LG (1991) Mental practice and memorization of piano music. J Gen Psychol 118:21–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Linden DEJ, Thornton K, Kuswanto CN, Johnston SJ, van de Ven V, Jackson MC (2011) The brain’s voices: comparing nonclinical auditory hallucinations and imagery. Cereb Cortex 21:330–337

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lotze M, Scheler G, Tan HRM, Braun C, Birbaumer N (2003) The musician’s brain: functional imaging of amateurs and professionals during performance and imagery. Neuroimage 20:1817–1829

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas BJ, Schubert E, Halpern AR (2010) Perception of emotion in sounded and imagined music. Music Percept 27:399–412

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukatela G, Turvey MT (1990) Phonemic similarity effect and prelexical phonology. Mem Cognit 18:128–152

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Macken WJ, Jones DM (1995) Functional characteristics of the inner voice and the inner ear: ­single or double agency? J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 21:436–448

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marks LE (1975) On colored-hearing synesthesia: cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions. Psychol Bull 82:303–331

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marks LE (1978) The Unity of the Senses: Interrelations among the Modalities. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay DG (1992) Constraints on theories of inner speech. In: Reisberg D (ed) Auditory Imagery Hillsdale. Erlbaum, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • McCusker LX, Hillinger ML, Bias RG (1981) Phonological recoding and reading. Psychol Bull 89:217–245

    Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen D, Perfetti C (1982) The visual tongue-twister effect: phonological activation in silent reading. J Verb Learn Verb Be 21:672–687

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire PK, Silbersweig DA, Murray RM, David AS, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD (1996) Functional anatomy of inner speech and auditory verbal imagery. Psychol Med 26:29–38

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McGuire PK, Silbersweig DA, Wright I, Murray RM, David AS, Frackowiak RSJ, Frith CD (1995) Abnormal monitoring of inner speech: a physiological basis for auditory hallucinations. Lancet 346:596–600

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meister IG, Krings T, Foltys H, Boroojerdi B, Müller M, Töpper R, Thron A (2004) Playing piano in the mind—an fMRI study on music imagery and performance in pianists. Cogn Brain Res 19:219–228

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mikumo M (1994) Motor encoding strategy for pitches of melodies. Music Percept 12:175–197

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills CB, Howell-Boteler E, Oliver GK (1999) Digit synaesthesia: a case study using a stroop-type task. Cogn Neuropsychol 16:181–191

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell RW, Gallaher MC (2001) Embodying music: matching music and dance in memory. Music Percept 19:65–85

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy SM, Nordin SM, Cumming J (2008) Imagery in sport, exercise and dance. In: Horn T (ed) Advances in Sport Psychology, 3rd edn. Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Nordin SM, Cumming J (2005) Professional dancers describe their imagery: where, when, what, why, and how. Sport Psychologist 19:395–416

    Google Scholar 

  • Nordin SM, Cumming J (2006) The development of imagery in dance. Part II: quantitative findings from a mixed sample of dancers. J Dance Med Sci 10:28–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Okada H, Matsuoka K (1992) Effects of auditory imagery on the detection of a pure tone in white noise: experimental evidence of the auditory Perky effect. Percept Mot Skills 74:443–448

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim GM, Dell GS (2008) Inner speech slips exhibit lexical bias, but not the phonemic similarity effect. Cognition 106:528–537

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim GM, Dell GS (2010) Motor movement matters: the flexible abstractness of inner speech. Mem Cognit 38:1147–1160

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio A (1971) Imagery and Verbal Processes. Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio A (1986) Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach. Oxford University Press, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmiero M, Olivetti Belardinelli M, Nardo D, Sestieri C, Di Matteo R, D’Ausilio A, Romani GL (2009) Mental imagery generation in different modalities activates sensory-motor areas. Cognit Processes 10(Suppl 2):S268–S271

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearce JMS (2007) Synaesthesia. Euro Neurol 57:120–124

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pechmann T, Mohr G (1992) Interference in memory for tonal pitch: implications for a working-memory model. Mem Cognit 20:314–320

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pecenka N, Keller PE (2009) Auditory pitch imagery and its relationship to musical synchronization. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1169:282–286

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pitt MA, Crowder RG (1992) The role of spectral and dynamic cues in imagery for musical timbre. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:728–738

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt DR (2006) Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspect Psychol Sci 1:110–122

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson LC, Sagiv N (eds) (2005) Synesthesia: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisberg D, Smith JD, Baxter DA, Sonenshine M (1989) “Enacted” auditory images are ambiguous; “pure” auditory images are not. Q J Exp Psychol 41A:619–641

    Google Scholar 

  • Repp BH (1997) Spectral envelope and context effects in the tritone paradox. Perception 26:645–665

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Repp BH, Goehrke RM (2011) Music notation, but not action on a keyboard, influences pianists’ judgments of ambiguous melodies. Music Percept 28:315–320

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudner M, Rönnberg J, Hugdahl K (2005) Reversing spoken items - mind twisting not tongue twisting. Brain Lang 92:78–90

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sabaté M, Llanos C, Rodriguez M (2008) Integration of auditory and kinesthetic information in motion: alterations in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 22:462–468

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salamé P, Baddeley AD (1982) Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech: implications for the structure of working memory. J Verb Learn Verb Be 21:150–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Sato M, Schwartz JL, Abry C, Cathiard MA, Loevenbruck H (2006) Multistable syllables as enacted perceptions: a source of an asymmetric bias in the verbal transformation effect. Percept Psychophys 68:458–474

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Satoh M, Takeda K, Nagata K, Hatazawa J, Kuzuhara S (2001) Activated brain regions in musicians during an ensemble: a PET study. Cogn Brain Res 12:101–108

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider TR, Engel AK, Debener S (2008) Multisensory identification of natural objects in a two-way cross-modal priming paradigm. Exp Psychol 55:121–132

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert E (2004) Modeling perceived emotion with continuous musical features. Music Percept 21:561–585

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubotz RI, Friederici AD, von Cramon DY (2000) Time perception and motor timing: a common cortical and subcortical basis revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 11:1–12

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sedlmeier P, Weigelt O, Walther E (2011) Music is in the muscle: how embodied cognition may influence music preferences. Music Percept 28:297–305

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal SJ, Fusella V (1970) Influence of imaged pictures and sounds on detection of visual and auditory signals. J Exp Psychol 83:458–464

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sharps MJ, Pollitt BK (1998) Category superiority effects and the processing of auditory images. J Gen Psychol 125:109–116

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sharps MJ, Price JL (1992) Auditory imagery and free recall. J Gen Psychol 119:81–87

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw G (2008) The multisensory image and emotion in poetry. Psychol Aesthetics Creativity Arts 2:175–178

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN, Cooper LA (eds) (1982) Mental Images and Their Transformations. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN, Podgorny P (1978) Cognitive processes that resemble perceptual processes. In: Estes WK (ed) Handbook of Learning and Cognitive Processes, vol 5. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Shergill SS, Bullmore E, Simmons A, Murray R, McGuire PK (2000) Functional anatomy of auditory verbal imagery in schizophrenic patients with auditory hallucinations. Am J Psychiatry 157:1691–1693

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simon O, Mangin JF, Cohen L, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S (2002) Topographical layout of hand, eye, calculation, and language-related areas in the human parietal lobe. Neuron 33:475–487

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Slotnick SD, Thompson WL, Kosslyn SM (2005) Visual mental imagery induces retinotopically organized activation of early visual areas. Cereb Cortex 15:1570–1583

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith JD, Wilson M, Reisberg D (1995) The role of subvocalization in auditory memory. Neuropsychologia 33:1433–1454

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C (2011) Crossmodal correspondences: a tutorial review. Atten Percept Psychophys 73:971–995

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spiller MJ, Jansari AS (2008) Mental imagery and synaesthesia: is synaesthesia from internally-generated stimuli possible? Cognition 109:143–151

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Theiler AM, Lippman LG (1995) Effects of mental practice and modeling on guitar and vocal performance. J Gen Psychol 122:329–343

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinti C, Cornoldi C, Marschark M (1997) Modality-specific auditory imaging and the interactive imagery effect. Eur J Cogn Psychol 9:417–436

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward J, Simner J, Auyeung V (2005) A comparison of lexical-gustatory and grapheme-colour synesthesia. Cogn Neuropsychol 22:28–41

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren RM (1968) Verbal transformation effect and auditory perceptual mechanisms. Psychol Bull 70:261–270

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weber RJ, Brown S (1986) Musical imagery. Music Percept 3:411–426

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson M, Knoblich G (2005) The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychol Bull 131:460–473

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winnick WA, Brody N (1984) Auditory and visual imagery in free recall. J Psychol 118:17–29

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zacks JM (2008) Neuroimaging studies of mental rotation: a meta-analysis and review. J Cogn Neurosci 20:1–19

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zatorre RJ, Halpern AR (1993) Effect of unilateral temporal-lobe excision on perception and imagery of songs. Neuropsychologia 31:221–232

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zatorre RJ, Halpern AR (2005) Mental concerts: musical imagery and the auditory cortex. Neuron 47:9–12

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zatorre RJ, Halpern AR, Bouffard M (2010) Mental reversal of imagined melodies: a role for the posterior parietal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 22:775–789

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zatorre RJ, Halpern AR, Perry DW, Meyer E, Evans AC (1996) Hearing in the mind’s ear: a PET investigation of musical imagery and perception. J Cogn Neurosci 8:29–46

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author thanks Andrea Halpern and Caroline Palmer for helpful comments on a previous version of this chapter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Timothy L. Hubbard .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hubbard, T.L. (2013). Auditory Imagery Contains More Than Audition. In: Lacey, S., Lawson, R. (eds) Multisensory Imagery. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-5879-1_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics