Skip to main content

Resistance to Interference: Developmental Changes in a Basic Processing Mechanism

  • Chapter
Emerging Themes in Cognitive Development

Abstract

Historically, theories of cognitive development have focused on two general processes: knowledge accumulation and information activation. Central to Piaget’s grand design, for example, is the notion that cognition is rule-governed (Reyna & Brainerd, 1991). He frequently stressed the importance of rule-based “operations” that, as they are acquired, progressively enlarge the child’s intellectual repertoire. Likewise, many more recent developments inspired by the rise of information-processing theories have explained age-related improvements in performance on the grounds that individuals acquire and implement more powerful rules, skills, or strategies (such as rehearsal, elaboration, and imagery) as they grow older (e.g., Fischer, 1980; Moely, 1977; Siegler, 1984). Although there are many points of divergence between Piagetian-type rules and information-processing rules, both traditions have nurtured the view that intellectual development is, in considerable part, a product of the accumulation of certain forms of knowledge.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Albert, M.S., & Kaplan, E. (1980). Organic implications of neuropsychological deficits in the elderly. In L.W. Poon, J.L. Fozard, L.S. Cermak, D. Arenberg, & L.W. Thompson (Eds.), New directions in memory and aging (pp. 403–432). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anooshian, L.J., & McCulloch, R.A. (1979). Developmental changes in dichotic listening with categorized word lists. Developmental Psychology, 15, 280–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R., DeVos, T., & Graber, M. (1989). Location memory in 8-month-old infants in a non-search AB task: Further evidence. Cognitive Development, 4, 345–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beech, A., Powell, T., McWilliams, J., & Claridge, G. (1989). Evidence of reduced “cognitive inhibition” in schizophrenia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28, 110–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berg, E.A. (1948). A simple objective test for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39, 378–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bianchi, L. (1895). The function of the frontal lobes. Brain, 18, 497–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork E.L., & Cummings, E.M. (1984). Infant search errors: Stage of concept development or stage of memory development. Memory & Cognition, 12, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R.A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In H.L. Roediger, III & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness (pp. 309–330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D.F. (1985). The role of conceptual knowledge in the development of organization in children’s memory. In C.J. Brained & M. Pressley (Eds.), Basic processes in memory development: Progress in cognitive development research (pp. 103–142). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D.F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children’s memory: An interpretive review. Developmental Review, 7, 93–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D.F., & Harnishfeger, K.K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10, 48–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J.S. (1966). On the conservation of liquids. In J.S. Bruner, R.R. Olver, & P.M. Greenfield, et al., (Eds.), Studies in cognitive growth (pp. 183–207). New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1974). Structures and strictures: Some functional limitations on the course of cognitive growth. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 544–573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chelune, G.J., & Baer, R.A. (1986). Developmental norms for the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neurospychology, 8, 219–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comalli, P.E., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects of Stroop Color-Word test in children, adulthood and aging. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 100, 47–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cragg, B.C. (1975). The density of synapses and neurons in normal, mentally defective and aging human brains. Brain, 98, 81–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R.G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R.G. (1989). Modularity and dissociations in memory systems. In H.L. Roediger, III. & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness (pp. 271–294). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, D.R., Jones, D.M., & Taylor, A. (1984). Selective and sustained-attention tasks: Individual and group differences. In R. Parasuraman, D.R. Davies, & J. Beatty (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 395–447). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (1981). Memory span: Sources of individual and developmental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 63–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (1985a). Proactive interference in sentence recall: Topic similarity effects and individual differences. Memory & Cognition, 13, 81–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (1985b). Short-term memory development in childhood and adolescence. In C.J. Brainerd & M. Pressley (Eds.), Basic processes in memory development (pp. 208–248). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (1989). Reflections on the nature and sources of individual differences in learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 1, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (in press). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (1991). Inhibitory processes: A neglected dimension of intelligence. Intelligence, 15, 157–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster F.N., & Cooney, J.B. (1982). Individual differences in digit span, susceptibility to proactive interference, and aptitude/achievement test scores. Intelligence, 6, 399–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (1988a). Abilities and neural mechanisms underlying AB̄ performance. Child Development, 59, 523–527.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (1988b). Differences between adult and infant cognition: Is the crucial variable presence or absence of language. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought without language (pp. 337–370). Oxford, England: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1986). Comparative development of human infants and infant rhesus monkeys of cognitive functions that depend on prefronal cortex. Neurosciences Abstracts, 12, 274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doyle, A.B. (1973). Listening to distraction: A development study of selective attention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 15, 100–115.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J.T., & Akhtar, N. (1989). A developmental study of filtering in visual attention. Child Development, 60, 1188–1199.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J.T., & Cameron, S. (1987). Selective attention in young children: The relations between visual search, filtering, and priming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 44, 38–63.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K.W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, M., & Oscar-Berman, M. (1986). Bilateral frontal lobe disease and selective delayed response deficits in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 337–342.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J.M. (1989). The prefrontal cortex (2nd ed.). New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. (1969). Conservation acquisition: A problem of learning to attend to relevant attributes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 7, 167–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1987). Development of cortical circuitry and cognitive function. Child Development, 58, 601–622.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gratch, G. (1976). On levels of awareness of objects in infants and students thereof. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 22, 157–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G.S. (1982). The development of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P.L. (1973). Perseverative errors in search by young infants. Child Development, 44, 28–33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R.T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–224). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, R.K. (1981). Wisconsin card sorting test manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D.O. (1939). Intelligence in man after large removals of cerebral tissue: Report of four left frontal lobe cases. Journal of General Psychology, 21, 73–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hecaen, H., & Albert, M.L. (1978). Human neuropsychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M.L., & Brainerd, C.J. (1989). Development of children’s long-term retention. Developmental Review, 9, 301–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M.L., & Rabinowitz, F.M. (1991). Gist another panacea? Or just the illusion of inclusion. Developmental Review, 11, 305–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudspeth, W.J., & Pribram, K.H. (1990). Stages of brain and cognitive maturation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 881–884.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, P.R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex— developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195–205.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hynd, G.W., & Willis, G. (1985). Neurological foundations of intelligence. In B.B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence: Theories, measurements, and applications (pp. 119–157). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janowsky, J.S., Shimamura, A.P., Kritchevsky, M., & Squire, L.R. (1989). Cognitive impairment following frontal lobe damage and its relevance to human amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 548–560.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A.R., & Rohwer, W.D., Jr. (1966). The Stroop Color-Word test: A review. Acta Psychologica, 25, 36–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R.V., Jr., & Le vine, L.A. (1976). Encoding processes and sex-role preferences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 256–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaMantia, A., & Rakic, P. (1984). The number, size, myelination and regional variations in the corpus callosum and anterior commissure of the developing rhesus monkey. Society of Neuroscience Abstracts, 10, 1373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lane, D.M., & Pearson, D.A. (1982). The development of selective attention. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 317–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, L. (1979, April). Mediation or production deficiency in disabled readers? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, P.H., & Norman, D.A. (1972). Human information processing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn, M.C. (1978). Influence of cognitive style and training on tasks requiring the separation of variables schema. Child Development, 49, 874–877.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loess, H., & Waugh, N.C. (1967). Short-term memory and inter-trial interval. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 455–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longmore, B.E., & Knight, R.G. (1988). The effect of intellectual deterioration on retention deficits in amnesic alcoholics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 448–454.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A.R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A.R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction for neuropsychology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGeoch, J. A. (1943). The psychology of human learning. New York: Longmans Green.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B. (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In J.M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior (pp. 313–334). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moely, B.E. (1977). Organizational factors in the development of memory. In R.V. Kail, Jr., & J.W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition (pp. 203–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B.B. (1974). Human memory: Theory and data. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oscar-Berman, M. (1980). Neuropsychological consequences of long-term chronic alcoholism. American Scientist, 68, 410–419.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parkin, A.J., & Leng, N.R.C. (1987). Aetiological variation in the amnesic syndrome. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris, & R.N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (Vol. 2, pp. 16–21). Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget’s developmental stages. Acta Psychologia, 63, 301–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). An organismic process model of Witkins’s field-dependence-independence. In T. Globerson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 36–70). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J., Ammon, P., Goodman, D., & Subleman, I. (1978). Piagetian theory and Neo-Piagetion analysis as psychological guides in education. In J.M. Gallagher & J. Easley (Eds.), Knowledge and development: Piaget and education (Vol. 2). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I.P. (1949). Complete Collected Works (Vols. 1–6). Moscow: Nauk SSSR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, L.R., & Peterson, M.J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perret, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behavior. Neuropsychologia, 12, 323–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plude, D.J., & Hoyer, W.J. (1985). Attention and performance: Identifying and localizing age deficits. In N. Charness (Ed.), Aging and human performance (pp. 47–99). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rand, G., Wapner, S., Werner, H., & McFarland, J.H. (1963). Age differences in performance on the Stroop Color-Word test. Journal of Personality, 31, 534–558.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reinis, S., & Goldman, J.M. (1980). The development of the brain. Sprinfield, II: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V.F., & Brainerd, C.J. (1991). Fuzzy-trace theory and children’s acquisition of mathematical and scientific concepts. Learning and Individual Differences, 3, 27–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rourke, B.P., Bakker, D.J., Fisk, J.L., & Stang, J.D. (1983). Child neuropsychology: An introduction to theory, research and clinical practice. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarnat, H.B., & Netsky, M.G. (1981). Evolution of the nervous system (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, L.L., & Moscovitch, M. (1984). Infants, amnesiacs, and dissociable memory systems. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Infant memory (pp. 173–216). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D.L., Moscovitch, M., Tulving, E., McLachlan, D.R., & Frendman, M. (1986). Mnemonic precedence in amnesiac patients: An analogue of the AB̄ error in infants. Child Development, 57, 816–823.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sejnowski, T.J., & Rosenberg, C.R. (1988). Learning and representation in connectionist models. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Perspectives in memory research (pp. 135–178). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R.S. (1984). Mechanisms of cognitive growth: Variation and selection. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.), Mechanisms of cognitive development (pp. 141–162). New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R.S. (1989). Mechanisms of cognitive development. In M.R. Rosenzweig & L.W. Porter (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 40, pp. 353–379). Palo Alto, CA. Annual Reviews.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sophian, C., Larking, J.H., & Kadane, J.B. (1985). A developmental model of search: Stochastic estimation of children’s rule use. In H.M. Wellman (Ed.), Children’s searching (pp. 185–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sophian, C., & Wellman, H.M. (1983). Selective information use and perseveration in the search behavior of infants and young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 369–390.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stamm, J.S. (1987). The riddle of the monkey’s delayed response deficit has been solved. In E. Perceman (Ed.), The frontal lobes revisited (pp. 73–89). New York: The IRBN Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strutt, G.F., Anderson, D.R., & Well, A.D. (1975). A developmental study of the effects of irrelevant information on speeded classification. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,20, 127–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D.T., & Benson, D.F. (1984). Neuropsychological Studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 3–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D.T., Benson, D.F., Kaplan, E.F., Weir, W.S., Naeser, M.A., Lieberman, I., & Ferrill, D. (1983). The involvement of orbitofrontal cerebrum in cognitive tasks. Neuropsychologia, 21, 235–248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D.T., Kaplan, E.F., Benson, D.F., Weir, W.S., Chivilli, S., & Sarazin, F.F. (1982). Evidence for the involvement of orbitofrontal cortex in memory fonctions: An interference effect. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 96, 913–925.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.L. (1964). The riddle of frontal lobe function in man. In J.M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior (pp. 410–444). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.L. (1972). Unity and diversity of frontal lobe functions. Acta Neurobiologica Experimenta, 32, 615–656.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S.P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S.P., Bourque, T.A., Anderson, S.H., & Brehaut, J.C. (1989). Mechanisms of attention: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 353–378.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waber, D. (1989). The biological boundaries of cognitive styles: A neuropsychological analysis. In T. Golberson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 11–35). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, L., Wall, A.J., & Anderson, L. (1967). Number conservation: The roles of reversibility, addition-subtraction, and misleading perceptual cues. Child Development, 38, 425–442.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, D.R. (1987). Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 660–669.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winer, G.A., Hemphill, J., & Craig, R.K. (1988). The effect of misleading questions in promoting nonconservation responses in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 24, 197–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wise, L.A., Sutton, J.A., & Gibbons, P.O. (1975). Decrement in Stroop interference time with age. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 41, 149–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witkin, H.A., Dyk, R.B., Faterson, G.E., Goodenough, D.R., & Karp, S.A. (1962). Psychological differentiation. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yakovlev, P.I., & LeCours, A.R. (1967). The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain. In A. Minkowski (Ed.), Regional development of the brain in early life (pp. 3–70). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelniker, T. (1989). Cognitive style and dimensions of information processing. In T. Globerson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 172–191). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimilies, H. (1963). A note on Piaget’s concept of conservation. Child Development, 34, 691–695.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1993 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Dempster, F.N. (1993). Resistance to Interference: Developmental Changes in a Basic Processing Mechanism. In: Howe, M.L., Pasnak, R. (eds) Emerging Themes in Cognitive Development. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9220-0_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9220-0_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-9222-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-9220-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics