Skip to main content

Affect and Learning: Causes and Consequences of Emotional Interactions

  • Chapter
Affect and Mathematical Problem Solving

Abstract

Affect is the least investigated aspect of human problem solving, yet it is probably the aspect most often mentioned as deserving further investigation. The “problem-solving” and “teaching-and-learning” literature is full of remarks that have a single message: “Someday soon — maybe tomorrow — we must get around to doing something about affect and emotion.” I am delighted to see that “tomorrow” has come.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Amsel, A. (1962). Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning. Psychological Review, 69, 306–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Averill, J.R. (1980). A constructivist view of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Theories of emotion (pp. 305–339). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berscheid, E. (1983). Emotion. In H.H. Kelley, E. Berscheid, A. Christensen, J.H. Harvey, T.L. Hudson, G. Levinger, E. McClintock, L.A. Peplau, & D.R. Peterson, Close relationships (pp. 110–168). San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G.H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G.H., & Mayer, J.D. (1985). In search of mood-dependent retrieval.Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D.E. (1971). Decision and stress. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C.R. (1980). The organization of action: A new synthesis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A.M., Means, B., Patrick, R., & Nowicki, G. (1982). Some factors influencing decision-making strategy and risk taking. In M.S. Clark & S.T. Fiske (Eds.), Affect and cognition: The 17th Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition (pp. 243–261). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R.S., Kanner, A.D., & Folkman, S. (1980). Emotions: A cognitive-phenomenological analysis. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Theories of emotion (pp. 189–217). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1959). Stimulus variables and subject variables: A caution. Psychological Review, 66, 145–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1972). Helplessness: Theory and research in anxiety. In C.D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research (Vol. 2, pp. 359–378). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and body: Psychology of emotion and stress. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Nakamura, Y. (1987). Aspects of consciousness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 299–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Sarason, S.B. (1952). A study of anxiety and learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 166–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Shebo, B.J. (1983). Knowing and liking. Motivation and Emotion, 7, 125–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Watson, D.L. (1966). Anxiety and the interruption of behavior. In C.D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety and behavior (pp. 263–288). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A. (1980). Errors in human performance (Report No. 8004). San Diego: University of California, Center for Human Information Processing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J.T. (1977). Skill and error in everyday life. In M. Howe (Ed.), Adult learning: Psychological research and applications (pp. 21–44). London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J.T. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatization. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness: Vol. 1, Psychological issues (pp. 67–89). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rilling, M. (1977). Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In W.K. Honig & J.E.R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 432–480). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A.H. (1985). Metacognitive and epistemological issues in mathematical understanding. In E. A. Silver (Ed.), Teaching and learning mathematical problem solving: Multiple research perspectives (pp. 361–379). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silver, E.A. (1985). Research on teaching mathematical problem solving: Some under-represented themes and needed directions. In E.A. Silver (Ed.), Teaching and learning mathematical problem solving: Multiple research perspectives (pp. 247–266). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B.F. (1961). Why we need teaching machines. Harvard Educational Review, 31, 377–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. A. (1953). A personality scale of manifest anxiety. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48, 285–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H.S. (1963). Discrimination learning with and without errors. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 1–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H.S. (1972). By-products of discrimination learning. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 5, pp. 195–265). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toda, M. (1982). Man, robot, and society. Boston: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wald, G. (1978). The human condition. In M.S. Gregory, A. Silver, & D. Sutch (Eds.), Sociobiology and human nature (pp. 277–282). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R.M., & Dodson, J.D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative and Neurological Psychology, 18, 459–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Mandler, G. (1989). Affect and Learning: Causes and Consequences of Emotional Interactions. In: McLeod, D.B., Adams, V.M. (eds) Affect and Mathematical Problem Solving. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-3614-6_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-3614-6_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-8178-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-3614-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics