Skip to main content

Abstract

The argument will be advanced in this paper that naive physics is neither a collection of unstructured knowledge elements nor a collection of stable misconceptions that need to be replaced,but rather a complex conceptual system that organises children’s perceptual experiences and information they receive from the culture into coherent explanatory frameworks that make it possible for them to function in the physical world.The processof learning science appears to be a slow and gradual one during which aspects of the scientific information are added on to the initial explanatory framework destroying its coherence until (and if)itis restructured in ways to make it consistent withcurrently accepted scientific views.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L.A. Hirschfeld and S.A. Gelman (Eds.) Mapping the mind (pp. 160–200). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M.T.H. (1992). Conceptual change within and across ontological categories: Examples from learning and discovery in science. In R. Giere (Ed.) Cognitive Models of Science: Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science. (pp. 129–160). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (1982). Students’ preconceptions in introductory mechanics. American Journal of Physics, 50, 66–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (July, 1986). Misconceptions in mechanics and an attempt to remediate them: The use of analogies and anchoring intuitions. Paper presented at the conference “The Psychology of Physics Problem Solving: Theory and Practice”, Bank Street College.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A.A. (1988). Knowledge in pieces. In G. Forman & P.B. Pufall (Eds.), Constructivism in the computer age (pp. 49–70). Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (1996). The scientist as a child. Philosophy of Science, 63, 485–514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1987). Everyday biology and school biology: How do they interact? The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 9, 120–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ioannides, C., & Vosniadou, S. (in press). Exploring the changing meanings of force: From coherence to fragmentation, Cognitive Science Quarterly.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ioannidou, I., & Vosniadou, S. (in press). The development of knowledge about the composition and layering of the earth’s interior. Nea Paideia (in Greek).

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Inhelder, B. (1974). If you want to go ahead get a theory. Cognition, 3, 195–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., Amsel, E., and O’ Longhlin, M., (1988). The development of scientific thinking skills. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kyrkos, C., & Vosniadou, S., (1997) Mental Models of Pant Nutrition. Poster presented at the Seventh European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction, Athens, Greece

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesley, A.M. (1994). ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In L.A. Hirschefeld, & S.A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M. (1983): Naive theories of motion. In D. Gentner & A.L. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Megalakaki, O., loannides, C., Vosniadou, S., & Tiberghien, A. (August, 1997). Differentiating force from energy: Children’s understanding of the concepts of energy and force. Seventh European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction, Athens, Greece.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ministrell, J. (1982). Explaining the “at rest” condition of an object. The Physics Teacher, January, 10–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J., & Resnick, L.B. (1989). Comparing historical and intuitive explanations of motion: Does “naive physics” have a structure? Proceedings of the 11thAnnual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 412–417). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

    Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, R. & Freyberg, P. (1985). Learning in science: The implications of childrens’ science. London: Heineman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ranney, M (1994). Relative consistency of subjects’ “Theories” in domains such as naive physics: Common research difficulties illustrated by Cooke and Breedin. Memory and Cognition, 22, 494–502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reif, F., & Allen, S. (1989). Interpreting and teaching scientific concepts: A study of acceleration. Cognition and Instruction, pp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiner, M., Slotta, J.D., Chi, M.T.H., and Resnick, L.B. (2000) Naive physics reasoning: A commitment to substance-based conceptions. Cognition and Instruction,18,1–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Springer, K., & Keil, F.C. (1989). On the development of biologically specific beliefs: The case of inheritance. Child Development, 60, 637–648.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J.P., diSessa, A.A., & Rochelle, J. (1993). Misconceptions Reconceived: A Constructivist analysis of knowledge in transition, The Journal of Learning Sciences, 3, 115–183

    Google Scholar 

  • Viennot, L. (1979). Spontaneous reasoning in elementary dynamics. European Journal of Science Education, 1, 205–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (1994). Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change. Learning and Instruction, 4, 45–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992): Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1994): Mental models of the day/night cycle. Cognitive Science, 18, 123–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., Ioannides, C., Dimitrakopoulou, A., & Papademitriou, E. (in press). Designing Learning Environments to Promote Conceptual Change in Science. Learning and Instruction

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiser, M. (1987). The differentiation of heat and temperature: History of science and novice-expert shift. In S. Strauss (Ed.), Ontogeny, phylogeny, and historical development. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2002 Kluwer Academic Publishers

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Vosniadou, S. (2002). On the Nature of Naïve Physics. In: Limón, M., Mason, L. (eds) Reconsidering Conceptual Change: Issues in Theory and Practice. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-47637-1_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-47637-1_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4020-0494-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-0-306-47637-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics