Skip to main content
Log in

Transitivity and non-uniform subjecthood in agreement attraction

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research on human language converges on a view in which a grammatical “subject” is the most saliently encoded entity in mental representation. However, subjecthood is not a syntactically uniform phenomenon. Notably, many languages encode morphological distinctions between subjects of transitive verbs (i.e., verbs that require an object) and subjects of intransitive verbs. We ask how this typological pattern manifests in a language like English (which does not morphologically signal it) by examining the “distinctiveness” of transitive versus intransitive subjects in memory during online sentence processing. We conducted a self-paced reading experiment that tested for “attraction” effects (Dillon et al., Journal of Memory and Language, 69(2), 85–103, 2013; Wagers et al., Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 206–237, 2009) in the processing of subject-verb number agreement. We find that transitive subjects trigger attraction effects, but that these effects are mitigated for intransitive subject attractors (independently of the number of other noun phrases present in the intervening clause). We interpret this as indicating that transitive subjects are less distinctive and therefore less representationally salient than intransitive subjects: This is because a transitive subject must compete with another clause-mate core argument (i.e., a direct object), which draws on resources from the same pool of memory resources. On the other hand, an intransitive subject minimally only competes with a non-core argument (i.e., an oblique noun phrase); this consumes fewer memory resources, leaving the subject to enjoy greater spoils.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In models of sentence processing, interference of an attractor can occur when it is encoded, so-called encoding interference (Barker et al., 2001; Hofmeister & Vasishth, 2014; Kush et al., 2015; Nairne, 1990; Oberauer & Lange, 2008; Villata et al., 2018), and then can affect retrieval (Laurinavichyute et al., 2017). For instance, if two NP items share overlapping features, the distinctiveness of these items in memory is reduced.

  2. One potential limitation of our design is a possible garden-path effect in intransitive conditions, because the verb and preposition were presented to participants separately. Therefore, it is possible that participants may have initially analyzed intransitive structures as being transitive, before the preposition was subsequently presented. However, since no interference effect was observed in intransitive structures (which we would expect if they had been treated as transitive), our results do not appear to wholly reflect such garden-path effects.

  3. It is potentially also noteworthy that the set of verbs used in the current study had subjects that fulfilled the thematic role of agent. Subjects characterized by non-agent semantic roles such as patient or experiencer might indeed manifest distinct behaviors. If this is the case, one might expect patient subjects of unaccusative verbs (e.g., fall, arrive) to trigger dissimilar attraction profiles to agents subjects of unergative verbs (e.g., dance, work), and/or for subjects of psychological predicates (e.g., love, see) to pattern differently to those of action predicates (e.g., chase, build). Likewise, inanimate subjects of intransitive verbs that permit pseudo-passives (e.g., “The couch was slept on”) might behave differently from animate subjects of non-passivized counterparts (e.g., “The child slept on the couch”), especially given that animacy is understood as a central component of agency. We leave these considerations open for future research.

References

  • Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1997). Young children’s productivity with word order and verb morphology. Developmental Psychology, 33, 952–965.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aldridge, E. C. (2004). Ergativity and word order in Austronesian languages. Cornell University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J., & Lebiere, C. (1998). The atomic components of thought. Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J., & Matessa, M. (1997). A production system theory of serial memory. Psychological Review, 104(4), 728–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariel, M. (1988). Referring and accessibility. Journal of Linguistics, 24, 65–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing noun-phrase antecedents. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnett, N., & Wagers, M. (2017). Subject encodings and retrieval interference. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 22–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, J. (2001). The effect of thematic roles on pronoun use and frequency of reference continuation. Discourse Processes, 31(2), 137–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, J. (2010). How speakers refer: The role of accessibility. Language and Linguistics Compass, 187– 203.

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Babyonyshev, M., & Gibson, E. (1999). The complexity of nested structures in Japanese. Language, 75, 423–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, J., Nicol, J., & Garrett, M. (2001). Semantic factors in the production of number agreement. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 91–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823.

  • Bhatia, S., & Dillon, B. (2022). Processing agreement in Hindi: When agreement feeds attraction. Journal of Memory and Language, 125, 104322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, K., & Miller, C. A. (1991). Broken agreement. Cognitive Psychology, 23(1), 45–93.

  • Bock, J. K., & Cutting, J. C. (1992). Regulating mental energy: Performance units in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 99–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, K., & Eberhard, K. M. (1993). Meaning, sound and syntax in English number agreement. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(1), 57–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosch, P., Katz, G., & Umbach, C. (2007). The non-subject bias of German. Anaphors in Text: Cognitive, Formal and Applied Approaches to Anaphoric Reference, 86, 145–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Box, G., & Cox, D. (1964). An analysis of transformations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 26(2), 211–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, J. (2001). Lexical Functional Syntax. Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W. L. (1976). Giveness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In C. N. Li (Ed.), Subject and Topic. Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, C. G., & Smyth, R. (1998). Structural parallelism and discourse coherence: A test of centering theory. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(4), 593–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax, vol. 11 of special technical reports of the research laboratory of electronics of the massachusetts institute of technology. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1982). Some concepts and consequences of the theory of government and binding. MIT press.

  • Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1993). Lectures on government and binding: The Pisa lectures (No. 9). Walter de Gruyter.

  • Clemens, L., Coon, J., Mateo Pedro, P., Morgan, A., Polinsky, M., Tandet, G., & Wagers, M. (2015). Ergativity and the complexity of extraction: A view from Mayan. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 33, 417–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawley, R. A., Stevenson, R. J., & Kleinman, D. (1990). The use of heuristic strategies in the interpretation of pronouns. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 19, 245–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunnings, I., & Sturt, P. (2018). Retrieval interference and semantic interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language, 102, 16–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dillon, B., Mishler, A., Sloggett, S., & Phillips, C. (2013). Contrasting intrusion profiles for agreement and anaphora: Experimental and modeling evidence. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(2), 85–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.04.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dillon, B., Andrews, C., Rotello, C. M., & Wagers, M. (2019). A new argument for co-active parses during language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(7), 1271–1286.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, R. M. W. (1979). Ergativity. Language, 55, 59–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, R. M. W. (1994). Ergativity. CUP.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Donkers, J., Hoeks, J. C. J., & Stowe, L. A. (2013). D-linking or set-restriction? Processing Which- questions in Dutch. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 9–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drummond, A. (2018). Ibex farm [Computer software manual]. http://spellout.net/ibexfarm. Accessed 2020.

  • Du Bois, J. (1987). The discourse basis of ergativity. Language, 805–855.

  • Engelmann, F., Jäger, L., & Vasishth, S. (2015). Cue confusion and distractor prominence can explain inconsistent interference effects. In 28th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing (p. 168). Los Angeles, CA: University of Southern California.

  • Engelmann, F., Jӓger, L. A., & Vasishth, S. (2019). The effect of prominence and cue association on retrieval processes: A computational account. Cognitive Science, 43(12), e12800.

  • Ershova, K. (2023). Ergatives are special: Parasitic resumptives and the Ergative Extraction Constraint in Samoan. In LSA 2023 Annual Meeting. Slides available at http://web.mit.edu/kershova/www/Ershova_LSA2023_slides.pdf

  • Ferreira, F., Bailey, K. G., & Ferraro, V. (2002). Good-enough representations in language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 11–15.

  • Ford, M. (1983). A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity throughout sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22(2), 203–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franck, J., Vigliocco, G., & Nicol, J. (2002). Subject-verb agreement errors in French and English: The role of syntactic hierarchy. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17(4), 371–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franck, J., & Wagers, M. (2020). Hierarchical structure and memory mechanisms in agreement attraction. Plos One, 15(5), e0232163.

  • Franck, J., Colonna, S., & Rizzi, L. (2015). Task-dependency and structure-dependency in number interference effects in sentence comprehension. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 132431.

  • Franck, J., Lassi, G., Frauenfelder, U. H., & Rizzi, L. (2006). Agreement and movement: A syntactic analysis of attraction. Cognition, 101(1), 173–216.

  • Franck, J., Soare, G., Frauenfelder, U. H., & Rizzi, L. (2010). Object interference in subject–verb agreement: The role of intermediate traces of movement. Journal of memory and language, 62(2), 166-182.

  • Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. (1996). Construal. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 1–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. (2000). The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In Y. Miyashita, A. P. Marantz, & W. O’Neil (Eds.), Image, language, brain (pp. 95–126). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P., Grosz, B., & Gilliom, L. (1993). Pronouns, names, and the centering of attention in discourse. Cognitive Science, 17, 311–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosz, B. J., Joshi, A. K., & Weinstein, S. (1995). Centering: A framework for modeling the local coherence of discourse. Computational Linguistics, 21, 203–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gundel, J. K. (2010). Reference and accessibility from a Givenness Hierarchy perspective. International Review of Pragmatics, 2, 148–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gundel, J. K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language, 69(2), 274–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammerly, C., Staub, A., & Dillon, B. (2019). The grammaticality asymmetry in agreement attraction reflects response bias: Experimental and modeling evidence. Cognitive psychology, 110, 70-104.

  • Hofmeister, P. (2011). Representational complexity and memory retrieval in language comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(3), 109–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmeister, P., & Norcliffe, E. (2014). Does resumption facilitate sentence comprehension? In P. Hofmeister & E. Norcliffe (Eds.), The Core and the Periphery: Data-Driven Perspectives on Syntax Inspired by Ivan A. Sag (pp. 225–246). CSLI Publ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofmeister, P., Vasishth, S. (2014). Distinctiveness and encoding effects in online sentence comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1237.

  • Holmes, V. M., & O’Regan, J. K. (1981). Eye fixation patterns during the reading of relative-clause sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20(4), 417–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, R. (2003). Two contributions of distinctive processing to accurate memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(4), 811–825.

  • Hunt, R. (2013). Precision in memory through distinctive processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 10–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonas, D., & Bobaljik, J. D. (1993). Specs for subjects: The role of TP in Icelandic. MIT working papers in linguistics, 18, 59–98.

  • Jurka, J. (2013). Subject islands in German revisited. In J. Sprouse & N. Hornstein (Eds.), Experimental syntax and Island effects (pp. 265–285). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, E. (2011). Salience and contrast effects in reference resolution: The interpretation of Dutch pronouns and demonstratives. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(10), 1587–1624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, E. L., & Comrie, B. (1977). Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 8(1), 63–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, E., & Comrie, B. (1979). Data on the noun phrase accessibility hierarchy. Language, 55(2), 333–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, E. L. (1976). Towards a universal definition of subject. Subject and topic, 303, 333.

  • Kempen, G., & Vosse, T. (1989). Incremental syntactic tree formation in human sentence processing: A cognitive architecture based on activation decay and simulated annealing. Connection Science, 1(3), 273–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, J., & Just, M. A. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 580–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kush, D., Johns, C. L., & van Dyke, J. (2015). Identifying the role of phonology in sentence-level reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 79, 18–29.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kwon, N., Polinsky, M., & Kluender, R. (2006, September). Subject preference in Korean. In Proceedings of the 25th west coast conference on formal linguistics (pp. 1-14).

  • Laurinavichyute, A., Jäger, L., Akinina, Y., Roß, J., Dragoy, O. (2017). Retrieval and encoding interference: Cross-linguistic evidence from anaphor processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 965.

  • Lewis, R. L., & Vasishth, S. (2005). An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science, 29(3), 375–419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, R. L., Vasishth, S., & Van Dyke, J. A. (2006). Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 10(10), 447–454.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, C.-J.C. (2018). Subject prominence and processing dependencies in prenominal relative clauses: The comprehension of possessive relative clauses and adjunct relative clauses in Mandarin Chinese. Language, 94(4), 758–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mak, W., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2006). Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(4), 466–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mccloskey, J. (1997). Subjecthood and subject positions. In L. Haegeman (Ed.), Elements of grammar (pp. 197–235). Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McElree, B. (2000). Sentence comprehension is mediated by content-addressable memory structures. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 111–123. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005184709695

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McElree, B., Foraker, S., & Dyer, L. (2003). Memory structures that subserve sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 67–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nairne, J. S. (1990). A feature model of immediate memory. Memory and Cognition, 18, 251–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, M., & Miyamoto, E. T. (2013). The object before subject bias and the processing of double-gap relative clauses in Japanese. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(3), 303–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ness, T., & Meltzer-Asscher, A. (2017). Working memory in the processing of long-distance dependencies: Interference and filler maintenance. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46, 1353–1365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ness, T., & Meltzer-Asscher, A. (2019). When is the verb a potential gap site? The influence of filler maintenance on the active search for a gap. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(7), 936–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicenboim, B., Vasishth, S., Engelmann, F., Suckow, K. (2018). Exploratory and confirmatory analyses in sentence processing: A case study of number interference in German. Cognitive Science, 42, 1075–1100.

  • Nicol, J. L., Forster, K. I., & Veres, C. (1997). Subject–verb agreement processes in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 36(4), 569–587.

  • O’Grady, W. (2011). Relative clauses: Processing and acquisition. In E. Kidd (Ed.), The acquisition of relative clauses: Processing, typology and function. UK: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberauer, K., & Lange, E. B. (2008). Interference in verbal working memory: Distinguishing similarity-based confusion, feature overwriting, and feature migration. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 730–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, D., & An, A. (2018). Not all phrases are equally attractive: Experimental evidence for selective agreement attraction effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patil, U., Vasishth, S., & Lewis, R. (2016). Retrieval interference in syntactic processing: The case of reflexive binding in English. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 329.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pearlmutter, N. J. (2000). Linear versus hierarchical agreement feature processing in comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 89–98.

  • Pearlmutter, N., Garnsey, S. M., & Bock, K. (1999). Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(3), 427–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polinsky, M., Gallo, C. G., Graff, P., Kravtchenko, E., Milton Morgan, A., & Sturgeon, A. (2013). Subject islands are different. In J. Sprouse & N. Hornstein (Eds.), Experimental syntax and island effects (pp. 286–309). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rohde, H., & Kehler, A. (2014). Grammatical and information-structural influences on pronoun production. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 29(8), 912–927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schriefers, H., Friederici, A. D., & Kuhn, K. (1995). The processing of locally ambiguous relative clauses in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(4), 499–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. (2011). Providing support for distinctive processing: The isolation effect in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 26(3), 744–751.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sturt, P., Kwon, N. (2017). Agreement attraction: Roles of active dependencies and attractor position. In: Paper Presented at the 30th CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing.

  • Theakston, A., Lieven, E., Pine, J., & Rowland, C. (2001). The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb-argument structure: An alternative account. Journal of Child Language, 28, 127–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tollan, R., & Heller, D. (2022). Pronoun resolution and ergativity: Effects of subjecthood and case in Niuean. Language, 98(1), 157–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, M. A., Idrissi, A., & Almeida, D. (2015). Representing number in the real- time processing of agreement: Self-paced reading evidence from Arabic. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 347.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Valian, V. (1986). Syntactic categories in the speech of young children. Developmental Psychology, 22(4), 562–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dyke, J. A. (2007). Interference effects from grammatically unavailable constituents during sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33, 407–430.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dyke, J. A., & Lewis, R. L. (2003). Distinguishing effects of structure and decay on attachment and repair: A cue-based parsing account of recovery from misanalyzed ambiguities. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 285–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dyke, J., & McElree, B. (2006). Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 157–166.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dyke, J., & McElree, B. (2011). Cue-dependent interference in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 247–263.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vasishth, S., Chen, Z., Li, Q., & Guo, G. (2013). Processing Chinese relative clauses: Evidence for the subject-relative advantage. PloS One, 10, e77006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Villata, S., Tabor, W., & Franck, J. (2018). Encoding and retrieval interference in sentence comprehension: Evidence from agreement. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2.

  • Villata, S., & Franck, J. (2020). Similarity-based interference in agreement comprehension and production: Evidence from object agreement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(1), 170–188.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagers, M., & McElree, B. (2009, September). Focal attention and the timing of memory retrieval in language comprehension. In Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (Vol. 15, pp. 7–9).

  • Wagers, M., Lau, E. F., & Phillips, C. (2009). Agreement attraction in comprehension: Representations and processes. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 206–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author Note

This research was presented at the Workshop on Linguistic Illusions in Sentence Processing (LISP), the Annual Conference on Human Sentence Processing 2022, KASELL Fall Conference on English Linguistics, and at research groups at the University of Delaware and the National University of Singapore. We thank these audiences for their valuable feedback. This research was developed from part of a Ph.D. dissertation by Myung Hye Yoo. All data, analysis code, and research materials are available at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/NS6V4. Funding for this project was received from the University of Delaware (awardee: Rebecca Tollan).

Funding

Funding for this project was received from the University of Delaware (awardee: Rebecca Tollan).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Myung Hye Yoo.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of interest

All authors certify that they have no affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest or non-financial interest in the subject matter or materials discussed in this article.

Ethics approval

This study was approved by the University of Delaware Internal Review Board (IRB).

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Open practices statement

All data, analysis code, and research materials are available at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/NS6V4.

The experiment was not preregistered.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yoo, M.H., Tollan, R. Transitivity and non-uniform subjecthood in agreement attraction. Mem Cogn 52, 536–553 (2024). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-023-01482-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-023-01482-8

Keywords

Navigation