Skip to main content
Log in

Optimized experimental designs to best detect spatial positional association of response codes in working memory

  • Published:
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The SPoARC (Spatial Positional Association of Response Codes) effect refers to spatialization of information in working memory. Among the potential factors that could influence how order is mapped onto a mental space during the recognition process, we selected the following two factors: i) the type of stimuli, in particular their verbal vs. visual aspects and ii) the number of probes. In this study, 137 participants memorized sequences of either words or pictures and subsequently performed a recognition test for which they responded using lateralized keys. For half of the participants, only one probe was presented after each sequence, whereas the other half was administered several probes. A significantly greater number of participants presented a SPoARC using a single probe. We discuss that spatialization is best detected when the sequence is scanned only once. Results also showed no difference between the two types of stimuli (i.e., verbal vs. visual). This finding raises the question of the respective roles of verbalization and visualization in the SPoARC.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Among these additional studies, we selected six studies subsequent to the literature review conducted by Guida and Campitelli (2019), but also two other studies published before the synthesis of Guida & Campitelli that we still considered relevant. Conversely, within the list of Guida & Campitelli, studies conducted by Rinaldi, Brugger, Bockisch, Bertolini, and Girelli (2015), Martarelli, Mast, and Hartmann (2017) and Antoine, Ranzini, Gebuis, van Dijck, and Gevers (2017) were excluded from our inventory because they did not measure spatialization using response times. The study carried out by Boiteau & Almor (2017, Experiment number two) was also discarded because of a protocol essentially focusing on the spatialization of syntax (the so-called syntax-space effect). This study was replaced by a study conducted by Boiteau (2015, Experiment number four) which had a more standard design and which allowed us to consider the effect of a new type of stimulus in our analysis. Finally, the study conducted by Wang, Liu, Shi, and Kang (2018) does not appear in our selection, as no regression coefficient was explicitly provided by the authors. Among the 21 studies listed in Table 1, study 1.c was discarded because the authors did not use a standard SPoARC paradigm to detect spatialization. Experiment 4.3, 4.4 and 5.1 were also discarded because they involved a SNARC effect (see Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux, 1993). In Boiteau (2015), we only selected Experiment number four because all other experiments involved the Syntax-Space effect. Study 9.1 was also discarded because spatialization was measured through whole-hand and precision grip instead of a standard key-pressing procedure commonly used to detect the SPoARC. Study 13.5 was excluded due to its use of articulatory suppression and tapping. Study 16.2 and 16.3 were excluded due to a vertical response mapping. Finally, 19.2 and 19.3 were not included due to their non central spatial coordinates.

  2. Some studies on the SPoARC effect allocated time to verify that the order of the different items was correctly memorized by participants, for example by asking participants to identify the memorized sequence among several ones at the end of each trial (for instance, the distractive sequences contained the same items organized in a different order; see Van Dijck and Fias, 2011 or Ginsburg et al., 2014).

  3. One participant was excluded from the analyses due to poor understanding of the instructions.

  4. Out of the 12 participants who were not native French speakers, four of them were native Arabic speaker, three native Spanish speaker, one native English speaker, one native Bulgarian speaker, one native Portuguese speaker, one native Albanian speaker and one native Tagalog speaker.

  5. https://www.freepik.com/free-vector/vector-illustration-juicy-raspberry-isolated-white_1215960.htm#page=1&query=raspberry&position=0&from_view=searchhttps://for raspberry, https://icones8.fr/icons/set/kiwi for kiwi, https://www.vecteezy.com/vector-art/3486476-doodle-freehand-sketch-drawing-of-blueberry-fruit for blueberry and https://fr.freepik.com/vectors-libre/fond-grenade-dessine-main_3885171.htm#page=1&query=grenade&position=20&from_view=search

  6. This effect was also significant when considering only the first probe presented after the to-be-recalled sequence in the multi-probe condition. This restricted analysis still showed slower reaction times in the mono-probe condition than in the multi-probe condition for the first probe (t(2801) = − 9.22, p < .001).

References

  • Abrahamse, E., Van Dijck, J.-P., Majerus, S., & Fias, W. (2014). Finding the answer in space: the mental whiteboard hypothesis on serial order in working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 932.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Annis, J., Malmberg, K. J., Criss, A. H., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2013). Sources of interference in recognition testing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(5), 1365.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Antoine, S., Ranzini, M., Gebuis, T., van Dijck, J.-P., & Gevers, W. (2017). Order information in verbal working memory shifts the subjective midpoint in both the line bisection and the landmark tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(10), 1973–1983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boiteau, T. W. (2015). The syntax-space effect (Doctoral dissertation, University of South Carolina).

  • Boiteau, T. W., & Almor, A. (2017). Transitivity, space, and hand: The spatial grounding of syntax. Cognitive Science, 41(4), 848–891.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonin, P., Poulin-Charronnat, B., Lukowski Duplessy, H., Bard, P., Vinter, A., Ferrand, L., & Méot, A. (2020). IMABASE: A new set of 313 colourised line drawings standardised in French for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, age-of-acquisition, and imageability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(11), 1862–1878.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bottini, R., Mattioni, S., & Collignon, O. (2016). Early blindness alters the spatial organization of verbal working memory. Cortex, 83, 271–279.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. R. (2004). Multimodel inference: Understanding AIC and BIC in model selection. Sociological Methods & Research, 33(2), 261–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, K. P., Anderson, D. R., & Huyvaert, K. P. (2011). AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: Some background, observations, and comparisons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65(1), 23–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cipora, K., van Dijck, J.-P., Georges, C., Masson, N., Goebel, S. M., Willmes, K., ..., Nuerk, H.-C. (2019). A minority pulls the sample mean: On the individual prevalence of robust group-level cognitive phenomena – the instance of the SNARC effect. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/bwyr3

  • Clark, S. E., & Gronlund, S. D. (1996). Global matching models of recognition memory: How the models match the data. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(1), 37–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Criss, A. H., Malmberg, K. J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2011). Output interference in recognition memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(4), 316–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Belder, M., Abrahamse, E., Kerckhof, M., Fias, W., & Van Dijck, J.-P. (2015). Serial position markers in space: Visuospatial priming of serial order working memory retrieval. PloS One, 10(1), e0116469.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, V., Archambeau, K., Van Dijck, J.-P., Chetail, F., & Gevers, W. (2017). Coding of serial order in verbal, visual and spatial working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146 (5), 632.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, V., Van Dijck, J.-P., Previtali, P., Fias, W., & Gevers, W. (2014). The impact of verbal working memory on number–space associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(4), 976.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, V, & Gevers, W (2015). Spatial coding of ordinal information in short-and long-term memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 8. Frontiers Media SA.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A., & Lavielle-Guida, M. (2014). 2011 space odyssey: Spatialization as a mechanism to code order allows a close encounter between memory expertise and classic immediate memory studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 573.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A., Gras, D., Noel, Y., Le Bohec, O., Quaireau, C., & Nicolas, S. (2013). The effect of long-term working memory through personalization applied to free recall: Uncurbing the primacy-effect enthusiasm. Memory & Cognition, 41(4), 571–587.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A., Leroux, A., Lavielle-Guida, M., & Noël, Y. (2016). A SPoARC in the dark: Spatialization in verbal immediate memory. Cognitive Science, 40(8), 2108–2121.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A., Megreya, A. M., Lavielle-Guida, M., Noël, Y., Mathy, F., Van Dijck, J.-P., & Abrahamse, E. (2018). Spatialization in working memory is related to literacy and reading direction: Culture “literarily” directs our thoughts. Cognition, 175, 96–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A, Carnet, S, Normandon, M, & Lavielle-Guida, M (2018). Can spatialisation be extended to episodic memory and open sets? Memory, 26(7), 922–935. Taylor & Francis.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A., & Campitelli, G. (2019). Explaining the SPoARC and SNARC effects with knowledge structures: An expertise account. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(2), 434–451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A., Abrahamse, E., & Van Dijck, J.-P. (2020). About the interplay between internal and external spatial codes in the mind: Implications for serial order. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1477(1), 20–33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A., Fartoukh, M., & Mathy, F. (2020). The development of working memory spatialization revealed by using the cave paradigm in a two-alternative spatial choice. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1477(1), 54–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guida, A., Mosinski, F., Cipora, K., Mathy, F., & Noël, Y. (2020). Spatialization in working memory: Can individuals reverse the cultural direction of their thoughts? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1477(1), 113–125.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heathcote, A. (2004). Fitting Wald and ex-Wald distributions to response time data: An example using functions for the S-PLUS package. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(4), 678–694.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huber, S, Klein, E, Moeller, K, & Willmes, K (2016). Spatial–numerical and ordinal positional associations coexist in parallel. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 438. Frontiers Media SA.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, M. S., Pike, R., Bain, J. D., & Tehan, G. (1989). Global matching: A comparison of the SAM, Minerva II, Matrix, and TODAM models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 33(1), 36–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • JASP Team, J. (2022). JASP (Version 0.16.1)[Computer software]. Retrieved from https://jasp-stats.org/

  • Lo, S., & Andrews, S. (2015). To transform or not to transform: Using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1171.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Martarelli, C. S., Mast, F. W., & Hartmann, M. (2017). Time in the eye of the beholder: Gaze position reveals spatial-temporal associations during encoding and memory retrieval of future and past. Memory & Cognition, 45(1), 40–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., ..., Lindeløv, J. K. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 195–203. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y

  • Pitt, M. A., & Myung, I. J. (2002). When a good fit can be bad. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(10), 421–425.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Previtali, P., De Hevia, M. D., & Girelli, L. (2010). Placing order in space: The SNARC effect in serial learning. Experimental Brain Research, 201(3), 599–605.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114 (3), 510.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reifman, A., & Keyton, K. (2010). Winsorize In Salkind NJ (ed.), encyclopedia of research design (pp. 1636–1638). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

  • Rinaldi, L., Brugger, P., Bockisch, C. J., Bertolini, G., & Girelli, L. (2015). Keeping an eye on serial order: Ocular movements bind space and time. Cognition, 142, 291–298.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rivest, L.-P. (1994). Statistical properties of Winsorized means for skewed distributions. Biometrika, 81(2), 373–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger III, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249–255.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shi, W., Wang, Q., Zhao, F., & Zhao, X. (2020). The spatial coding mechanism of ordinal symbols: A study based on the ordinal position effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(3), 1051–1062.

    Google Scholar 

  • Team, R. C., & et al (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing.

  • Van Dijck, J.-P., & Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations. Cognition, 119(1), 114–119.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijck, J.-P., Abrahamse, E. L., Majerus, S., & Fias, W. (2013). Spatial attention interacts with serial-order retrieval from verbal working memory. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1854–1859.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijck, J.-P., Abrahamse, E. L., Acar, F., Ketels, B., & Fias, W. (2014). A working memory account of the interaction between numbers and spatial attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1500–1513.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijck, J-P, Fias, W, & Andres, M (2015). Selective interference of grasp and space representations with number magnitude and serial order processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1370–1376. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijck, J.-P., Abrahamse, E., & Fias, W. (2020). Do preliterate children spontaneously employ spatial coding for serial order in working memory? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1477(1), 91–99.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Q., Liu, M., Shi, W., & Kang, J. (2018). Mechanism of the SNARC effect in numerical magnitude, time sequence, and spatial sequence tasks: Involvement of LTM and WM. Frontiers in Psychology, p 1558.

  • Yates, F. A. (1966). The Art of Memory. Chicago IL: The University of Chicago Press.

  • Zhou, D., Cai, Q., Luo, J., Yi, Z., Li, Y., Seger, C. A., & Chen, Q. (2021). The neural mechanism of spatial-positional association in working memory: A fMRI study. Brain and Cognition, 152, 105756.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, D., Zhong, H., Dong, W., Li, M., Verguts, T., & Chen, Q. (2019). The metaphoric nature of the ordinal position effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(8), 2121–2129.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Fabien Mathy.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Open practices statement

The experiments were not preregistered. The data for the experiment and the R scripts are available at https://osf.io/fam6q/.

Author Note

This research was supported in part by a grant from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR-17-CE28-0013-01) awarded to Fabien Mathy and a grant (Boost’Europe-21004278-00001052) from Region Bretagne awarded to Alessandro Guida. The first co-authors Morgane Ftaïta and Maëliss Vivion (by alphabetical order) contributed equally to this work. Conceptualization, all authors; Methodology, Morgane Ftaïta, Alessandro Guida, Fabien Mathy, Maëliss Vivion; Formal Analysis, Morgane Ftaïta, Alessandro Guida, Fabien Mathy, Maëliss Vivion; Writing, all authors.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

(PDF 143 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g., a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ftaïta, M., Vivion, M., Banks, E. et al. Optimized experimental designs to best detect spatial positional association of response codes in working memory. Atten Percept Psychophys 85, 1661–1680 (2023). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-023-02666-9

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-023-02666-9

Keywords

Navigation