Skip to main content
Log in

Primate Innovation: Sex, Age and Social Rank Differences

  • Published:
International Journal of Primatology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Analysis of an exhaustive survey of primate behavior collated from the published literature revealed significant variation in rates of innovation among individuals of different sex, age and social rank. We searched approximately 1,000 articles in four primatology journals, together with other relevant databases, for examples of innovation. The reported incidence of innovation is higher in males and adults, and lower in females and nonadults, than would be expected by chance given the estimated relative proportions of these groups. Amongst chimpanzees, the only species for which there are sufficient data to consider alone, there is a similar sex difference in the propensity to innovate, but no effect of age. Chimpanzees of low social rank are reported as innovators more frequently than high-ranking chimpanzees are. Male chimpanzees innovate more often than females in sexual, courtship, mating and display contexts; that is, in contexts likely to increase access to mates. The largest number of recorded observations are in the foraging context, wherein contrary to expectations, there is no evidence for female chimpanzees exhibiting more innovation than males. The study is the first extensive investigation of behavioral innovation in primates and provides evidence that much individual variation in the propensity to innovate can be explained in terms of sex, age, and social rank.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Social Learning Theory, Prentice-Hall, New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bean, A. (1999). Ecology of sex differences in great ape foraging. In Lee, P.C. (ed.), Comparative Primate Socioecology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 339-362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (1991). Teaching amongst wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 41: 530-532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (1999). A theory that's hard to digest. Nature 399: 653.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. (1981). Sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees: a preliminary report. Journal of Human Evolution 10: 585-593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. (1984). Possible causes of sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution 13: 415-440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boinski, S. (1988). Use of a club by a wild white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus) to attack a venomous snake (Bothrops asper). American Journal of Primatology 14: 177-179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Box, H. O. (1991). Training for life after release: simian primates as examples. Symposia of the Zoological Society of London 62: 111-123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Box, H. O. (1997). Foraging strategies among male and female marmosets and tamarins (Callitrichidae): new perspectives in an underexplored area. Folia Primatologica 68: 296-306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunnell, B. N., Gore, W. T., and Perkins, M. N. (1980). Performance correlates of social behaviour and organization: social rank and reversal learning in crab-eating macaques (M. fascicularis). Primates 21: 376-388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunnell, B. N., and Perkins, M. N. (1980). Performance correlates of social behaviour and organization: social rank and complex problem solving in crab-eating macaques (M. fascicularis). Primates 21: 515-523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. (1993). Do larger brains mean greater intelligence? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 696-697.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W., and Whiten, A. (1992). Cognitive evolution: evidence from tactical deception. Man 27: 609-627.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cambefort, J. P. (1981). A comparative study of culturally transmitted patterns of feeding habits in the chacma baboon Papio ursinus and the vervet monkey Cercopithecus aethiops. Folia Primatologica 36: 243-263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daly, M., and Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, Evolution and Behavior, 2nd edn, Wadsworth, Belmont, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, A. F. (1998). Primate Sexuality: Comparative Studies of the Prosimians, Monkeys, Apes and Human Beings, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1987). Demography and reproduction. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds.), Primate Societies, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 240-249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaton, G. G. (1976). The social order of Japanese macaques. Scientific American 234(4): 96-106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fragaszy, D., and Visalberghi, E. (1989). Social influences on the acquisition of tool-using behaviors in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology 103(2): 159-170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galdikas, B. M. F. (1982). Orang-utan tool-use at Tanjung Puting reserve, central Indonesian Borneo (Kalimanta Tengah). Journal of Human Evolution 10: 19-33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geissmann, T., and Braendle, C. (1997). Helping behaviour in captive pileated gibbons. Folia Primatologica 68: 110-112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behaviour, Belknap Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannah, A. C., and McGrew, W. C. (1987). Chimpanzees using stones to crack open oil palm nuts in Liberia. Primates 28(1): 31-46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, P. J. B. (1993). Teleost foraging: facts and theories. In Pitcher, T. J. (ed.), Behaviour of Teleost Fishes, 2nd edn., Chapman & Hall, London, pp. 253-284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, P. H., and Pagel, M. D. (1991). The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. (1988). Invention and social transmission: new data from wild vervet monkeys. In Byrne, R.W., and Whiten, A. (eds.), Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 327-343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hosey, G. R., Jacques M., and Pitts A. (1997). Drinking from tails: Social learning of a novel behaviour in a group of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Primates 38: 415-422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howell, D. C. (1997). Statistical Methods for Psychology, Fourth Edition, Duxbury, Belmont.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, T. D. (1982). The selective costs and benefits of learning: an evolutionary analysis. Advances in the Study of Behaviour 12: 65-106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawai, M. (1965). Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islet. Primates 6(1): 1-30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler,W. (1925). The Mentality of Apes, Kegan Paul, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kummer, H. (1971). Primate societies: Group techniques of ecological adaptation, Aldine, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kummer, H., and Goodall, J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B 308: 203-214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N. (1992). A theoretical investigation of the role of social transmission in evolution. Ethology and Sociobiology 13: 87-113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., and Reader, S. M. (1999a). Foraging innovation in the guppy. Animal Behaviour 57: 331-340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., and Reader, S. M. (1999b). Foraging innovation is inversely related to competitive ability in male but not in female guppies. Behavioral Ecology 10: 270-274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. (1996). Developing a theory of animal social learning. In Heyes, C. M., and Galef, B. G., Jr. (eds.), Social Learning in Animals: the Roots of Culture, Academic Press, London, pp. 129-154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, P. (1991). Adaptations to environmental change: An evolutionary perspective. In Box, H. O. (ed.), Primate Responses to Environmental Change, Chapman & Hall, London, pp. 39-56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefebvre, L., Gaxiola, A., Dawson, S. et al. (1998). Feeding innovations and forebrain size in Australasian birds. Behaviour 135: 1077-1097.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefebvre, L., Whittle, P., Lascaris, E., and Finkelstein, A. (1997). Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Animal Behaviour 53: 549-560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, R. (1995). Birth of a tool-maker. New Scientist 11 March: 38-41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ludes, E., and Anderson, J. R. (1995). Peat-bathing by captive white faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Folia Primatologica 65: 38-42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, P. R., and Bateson, P. (1986). Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masters, J. C., Centner, M. R., and Caithness, N. (1982). Sex ratios in Galagos revisited. South African Journal of Science 78: 198-202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masters, J. C., Lumsden, W. H. R., and Young, D. A. (1993). Tertiary sex ratios in wild Galago populations. Journal of Zoology, London 230: 659-663.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, W. C. (1979). Evolutionary implications of sex differences in chimpanzee predation and tool use. In Hamburg, D. A., and McCown, E. R. (eds.), The Great Apes, Benjamin/Cummings, Menlo Park, California, pp. 441-463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milinski, M. (1993). Predation risk and feeding behaviour. In Pitcher, T. J. (ed.), Behaviour of Teleost Fishes, 2nd edn, Chapman & Hall, London, pp. 285-306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C., Gros-Louis, J., and Richards, A. F. (1996). Sexual dimorphism, the operational sex ratio, and the intensity of male competition in polygynous primates. The American Naturalist 147(6): 966-980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishida, T., and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. (1987). Chimpanzees and bonobos: cooperative relationships among males. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds.), Primate Societies, University of Chicago, Chicago, pp. 165-178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pagel, M. (1999). Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature 401: 877-884.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M. (1999). Social Learning and Innovation: Individual Differences, Diffusion Dynamics and Evolutionary Issues. PhD thesis, University of Cambridge.

  • Rogers, E. M. (1995). Diffusion of Innovations, 4th edn, Free Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, N. (1996). The Pictorial guide to the Living Primates, Pogonias Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigg,H. (1980). Differentiation of female positions in hamadryas one-male units. Zietschrift für Tierpsychologie 53: 265-302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B. (1987). Social behaviour in evolutionary perspective. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R.W., and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds.), Primate Societies, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 318-329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T. (1987). Primate Societies, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sol, D., and Lefebvre L. (2000). Forebrain size and foraging innovations predict invasion success in birds introduced to New Zealand. Oikos 90: 599-605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strayer, F. F. (1976). Learning and imitation as a function of social status in Macaque monkeys (Macaca nemestrina). Animal Behaviour 24: 835-848.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sulloway, F. (1996). Born to Rebel, Little, Brown & Co, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth, N., Schick K. D., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Sevcik R. A., and Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993).Pan the tool-maker: Investigations into the stone tool-making and tool-using capabilities of a bonobo (Pan paniscus). Journal of Archaeological Science 20: 81-91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsumori, A. (1967). Newly acquired behaviour and social interactions of Japanese monkeys. In Altmann, S. A. (ed.), Social Communication Among Primates, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 207-219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, K. (1989). Fish: a new addition to the diet of Japanese macaques on Koshima Island. Folia Primatologica 52: 124-131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., and Byrne, R. (1988). Taking (Machiavellian) intelligence apart: editorial. In Byrne, R. W., and Whiten, A. (eds.), Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 50-65.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Reader, S.M., Laland, K.N. Primate Innovation: Sex, Age and Social Rank Differences. International Journal of Primatology 22, 787–805 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012069500899

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012069500899

Navigation