Skip to main content
Log in

First-grade multilingual students’ executive function profiles and links to English reading achievement and difficulties: a person-centered latent profile analysis

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although the important role of children’s executive function (EF) in their reading development has been well-established, less is known about the extent to which multilingual children’s EF components vary and whether the variability in different EF abilities explains multilingual children’s English reading achievement. The present study explored the US first-grade multilingual children’s (N = 3,819) profiles of EF abilities and how the profile membership was associated with their English reading achievement, using a nationally representative sample of multilingual children from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 2010–11 (ECLS-K: 2011) study. We fit latent profile analysis with various EF components, including working memory, cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, approaches to learning, and attentional focus, and found three distinct EF profiles in multilingual children: (a) Below-Average EFs with Above-Average Cognitive Flexibility (10.13%), (b) Above-Average EFs (84.09%), and (c) Very Low Cognitive Flexibility (5.78%). Controlling for kindergarten English reading achievement scores and demographic variables, children in the Above-Average EFs profile attained a significantly higher English reading achievement score than their peers, while children in the Very Low Cognitive Flexibility group had the lowest English reading achievement score. Our findings highlight the importance of understanding multilingual children’s heterogeneity in EF and have implications for the early identification of and tailored intervention for multilingual children at risk for reading difficulties.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adi-Japha, E., Berberich-Artzi, J., & Libnawi, A. (2010). Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development, 81, 1356–1366. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01477.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Al Otaiba, S., & Fuchs, D. (2002). Characteristics of children who are unresponsive to early literacy intervention: A review of the literature. Remedial and Special Education, 23(5), 300–316. https://doi.org/10.1177/07419325020230050501

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altemeier, L. E., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2008). Executive functions for reading and writing in typical literacy development and dyslexia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(5), 588–606. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390701562818

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 21(3), 329–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1986).Working memory. Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.

  • Bakk, Z., & Vermunt, J. K. (2016). Robustness of stepwise latent class modeling with continuous distal outcomes. Structural Equation Modelling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 23, 20–31. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2014.955104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 699–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. J., & Curran, P. J. (2004). The Integration of continuous and discrete latent variable models: Potential problems and promising opportunities. Psychological Methods, 9(1), 3–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.9.1.3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, D. R., Miao, A., Duncan, R., & McClelland, M. M. (2014). Behavioral self-regulation and executive function both predict visuomotor skills and early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 411–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergman, L. R., Magnusson, D., & El-Khouri, B. M. (2003). Studying individual development in an interindividual context: A personoriented approach. Erlbaum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V., & Nagy, W. (2008). Flexibility in word reading: Multiple levels of representations, complex mappings, partial similarities, and cross-modality connections. In K. Cartwright (Ed.), Literacy processes: Cognitive flexibility in learning and teaching (pp. 114–139). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berthelsen, D., Hayes, N., White, S. L., & Williams, K. E. (2017). Executive function in adolescence: Associations with child and family risk factors and self-regulation in early childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 903. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00903

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berthiaume, K. S., Lorch, E. P., & Milich, R. (2010). Getting clued in: Inferential processing and comprehension monitoring in boys with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 14(1), 31–42. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054709347197

  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81, 1637–1640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 240–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9(1), 103–119. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069050090010701

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biggs, J. B. (1987). Student approaches to learning and studying. Australian Council for Educational Research.

  • Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647–663. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 105–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterfuss, R., & Kendeou, P. (2018). The role of executive functions in reading comprehension. Educational Psychology Review, 30(3), 801–826. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-017-9422-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: A meta-analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 246–251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.10.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cartwright, K. B. (2015). Executive skills and reading comprehension: A guide for educators. Guilford Press.

  • Cartwright, K. B., Coppage, E. A., Lane, A. B., Singleton, T., Marshall, T. R., & Bentivegna, C. (2017). Cognitive flexibility deficits in children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 50, 33–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.01.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cartwright, K. B., Marshall, T. R., Dandy, K. L., & Isaac, M. C. (2010). The development of graphophonological-semantic cognitive flexibility and its contribution to reading comprehension in beginning readers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11(1), 61–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248370903453584

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cartwright, K. B., Marshall, T. R., Huemer, C. M., & Payne, J. B. (2019). Executive function in the classroom: Cognitive flexibility supports reading fluency for typical readers and teacher-identified low-achieving readers. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 88, 42–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2019.01.011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P., Siegel, L. S., & Hasher, L. (2000). Working memory, inhibitory control, and reading disability. Memory & Cognition, 28, 8–17. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211570

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christopher, M. E., Miyaka, A., Keenan, J. M., Pennington, B., DeFries, J. C., Wadsworth, S. J., Willcutt, E., & Olson, R. K. (2012). Predicting word reading and reading comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 470–488. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027375

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Beni, R., & Palladino, P. (2000). Intrusion errors in working memory tasks: Are they related to reading comprehension ability? Learning and Individual Differences, 12(2), 131–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(01)00033-4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiPerna, J. C., Volpe, R. J., & Elliott, S. N. (2002). A model of academic enablers and elementary reading/language arts achievement. School Psychology Review, 31(3), 298–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dolch, E. W. (1948). Problems in reading. Champaign, IL: Garrard Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebert, K. D., & Pham, G. (2019). Including nonlinguistic processing tasks in the identification of developmental language disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28(3), 932–944. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_AJSLP-IDLL-18-0208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Espy, K. A., & Bull, R. (2005). Inhibitory processes in young children and individual variation in short-term memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 669–688. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrar, M. J., & Ashwell, S. (2012). Phonological awareness, executive functioning, and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 27, 77–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.08.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flory, K., Molina, B. S. G., Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E., & Smith, B. (2006). Childhood ADHD predicts risky sexual behavior in young adulthood. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 571–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Follmer, D. J. (2018). Executive function and reading comprehension: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist, 53(1), 42–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2017.1309295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, K. L., Cabell, S. Q., Konold, T. R., Invernizzi, M., & Gartland, L. R. (2013). Diversity among Spanish-speaking English language learners: Profiles of early literacy skills in kindergarten. Reading and Writing, 26(6), 889–912. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9397-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuhs, M. W., Farran, D. C., & Nesbitt, K. T. (2015). Prekindergarten children’s executive functioning skills and achievement gains: The utility of direct assessments and teacher ratings. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 207–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037366

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., Varner, K. R., & Faust, M. (1990). Investigating differences in general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 430–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, S., & Naglieri, J. A. (Eds.). (2014). Handbook of executive functioning. Springer Science + Business Media. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5

  • Goodrich, J. M., Koziol, N. A., Yoon, H., & Leiva, S. (2022). Do Spanish-English bilingual children outperform monolingual English-speaking children on executive function tasks in early childhood? A propensity score analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 114(4), 870–892. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000692

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67–81. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728998000133

  • Gunnerud, H. L., Ten Braak, D., Reikerås, E., Donolato, E., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2020). Is bilingualism related to a cognitive advantage in children? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(12), 1059–1083. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hautala, J., Loberg, O., Azaiez, N., Taskinen, S., Tiffin-Richards, S. P., & Leppänen, P. H. T. (2019). What information should I look for again? Attentional difficulties distracts reading of task assignments. Learning and Individual Differences, 75, 101775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2019.101775

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L., Snowling, M., & Clark, P. (2013). Accessing, integrating, and inhibiting word meaning in poor comprehenders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 177–198. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2011.652721

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625–658. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, R., & Parkinson, J. (2015). The potential for school-based interventions that target executive function to improve academic achievement: A review. Review of Educational Research, 85(4), 512–552. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654314561338

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., Helder, A., & Karlsson, J. (2014). A cognitive view of reading comprehension: Implications for reading difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 29(1), 10–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/ldrp.12025

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kids Count Data Center. (2019). Children in immigrant families in the United States. Retrieved from https://datacenter.kidscount.org/data/tables/115-children-in-immigrant-families?loc=1&loct=1#detailed/1/any/false/1729,37,871,870,573,869,36,868,867,133/any/445,446

  • Kieffer, M. J. (2008). Catching up or falling behind? Initial English proficiency, concentrated poverty, and the reading growth of language minority learners in the United States. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(4), 851–868. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.4.851

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kieffer, M. J., Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Logan, J. K. (2021). Executive functions and English reading comprehension growth in Spanish-English bilingual adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 73, 101238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2021.101238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kieffer, M. J., Vukovic, R. K., & Berry, D. (2013). Roles of attention shifting and inhibitory control in fourth-grade reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 333–348. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (2005). Insights into second language reading: A cross-linguistic approach. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139524841

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497–514. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.799170

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6(2), 293–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., & Geva, E. (2006). Synthesis: Development of literacy in language-minority students. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on language-minority children and youth (pp. 53–74). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li-Grining, C. P., Votruba-Drzal, E., Maldonado-Carreño, C., & Haas, K. (2010). Children’s early approaches to learning and academic trajectories through fifth grade. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1062–1077. https://doi.org/10.1037/a002006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locascio, G., Mahone, E. M., Eason, S. H., & Cutting, L. E. (2010). Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension deficits. Journal of Learning Disabilities., 43, 441–454. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219409355476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H., Belletier, C., & Doherty, J. M. (2021). Integrating theories of working memory. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martinussen, R., & Mackenzie, G. (2015). Reading comprehension in adolescents with ADHD: Exploring the poor comprehender profile and individual differences in vocabulary and executive functions. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 38, 329–337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.12.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, K. S., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). WJ III technical manual. Riverside.

  • Miller, A. C., Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., & Olson, R. K. (2013). Reading comprehension in children with ADHD: Cognitive underpinnings of the centrality deficit. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(3), 473–483. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9686-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (Eds.). (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139174909

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 187–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2017). Mplus User's Guide. Eighth Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

  • Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. K. (2000). Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: Growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(6), 882–891. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb02070.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Najarian, M., Tourangeau, K., Nord, C., & Wallner-Allen, K. (2018). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 2010–11 (ECLS-K:2011), First- and Second-Grade Psychometric Report (NCES 2018–183). National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.

  • National Center for Education Statistics. (2019). The condition of education: English language learners in public schools. Retrieved July 30, 2021, from https://www.nationsreportcard.gov/reading/nation/achievement/?grade=4

  • National Research Council. (2008). In C. E. Snow & S. B. Van Hemel (Eds.), Early childhood assessment: Why, what, and how. Board on Children, Youth, and Families, Board on Testing and Assessment, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education: The National Academies Press.

  • Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(4), 535–569. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701575396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J., & Yuill, N. (1986). Pronoun resolution in skilled and less-skilled comprehenders: Effects of memory load and inferential complexity. Language and Speech, 29(1), 25–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ober, T. M., Brooks, P. J., Homer, B. D., & Rindskopf, D. (2020). Executive functions and decoding in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic investigation. Educational Psychology Review, 32(3), 735–763. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09526-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oberski, D. L. (2016). Mixture models: Latent profile and latent class analysis. In J. Robertson, & M. Kaptein (Eds.), Modern statistical methods for HCI (pp. 275–287). (Human–Computer Interaction Series). Springer.https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-26633-6_12

  • Peng, P., & Kievit, R. A. (2020). The development of academic achievement and cognitive abilities: A bidirectional perspective. Child Development Perspectives, 14(1), 15–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, P., Barnes, M., Wang, C., Wang, W., Li, S., Swanson, H. L., Dardick, W., & Tao, S. (2018). A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 48–76. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pliszka, S. R., Borcherding, S. H., Spratley, K., Leon, S., & Irick, S. (1997). Measuring inhibitory control in children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 18(4), 254–259. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004703-199708000-00005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(1), 102–112. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, J. M., & Wagner, R. K. (2018). Using meta-analytic structural equation modeling to study developmental change in relations between language and literacy. Child Development, 89(6), 1956–1969. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13049

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raudszus, H., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2018). Lexical quality and executive control predict children’s first and second language reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 31(2), 405–424. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9791-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Relyea, J. E., & Amendum, S. J. (2020). English reading growth in Spanish-speaking bilingual students: Moderating effect of English proficiency on cross-linguistic influence. Child Development, 91(4), 1150–1165. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13288

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, G., Mohammed, S. S., & Vaughn, S. (2010). Reading achievement across three language groups: Growth estimates for overall reading and reading subskills obtained with the Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 668–686. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018983

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, S. A., Geldhof, G. J., Purpura, D. J., Duncan, R., & McClelland, M. M. (2017). Examining the relations between executive function, math, and literacy during the transition to kindergarten: A multi-analytic approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(8), 1120–1140. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schrank, F. A., McGrew, K. S., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). Technical abstract (Woodcock-Johnson III Assessment Service Bulletin No. 2). Riverside Publishing.

  • Simon, J. R., & Wolf, J. D. (1963). Choice reaction time as a function of angular stimulus-response correspondence and age. Ergonomics, 6(1), 99–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140136308930679

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, M., Richmond, M. C., & Cutting, L. E. (2020). Considering the role of executive function in reading comprehension: A structural equation modeling approach. Scientific Studies of Reading, 24(3), 179–199. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2019.1643868

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., Sáez, L., & Gerber, M. (2006). Growth in literacy and cognition in bilingual children at risk or not at risk for reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 247–264. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taboada Barber, A., Cartwright, K. B., Hancock, G. R., & Klauda, S. L. (2021). Beyond the simple view of reading: The role of executive functions in emergent bilinguals’ and English monolinguals’ reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 56(S1), S45–S64. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.385

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taboada Barber, A., Cartwright, K. B., Stapleton, L., Klauda, S. L., Archer, C., & Smith, P. (2020). Direct and indirect effects of executive functions, reading engagement, and higher order strategic process in the reading comprehension of dual language learners and English monolinguals. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101848

  • Tourangeau, K., Nord, C., Lê, T., Wallner-Allen, K., Hagedorn, M.C., Leggitt, J., & Najarian, M. (2015). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 2010–11 (ECLS-K:2011), User’s Manual for the ECLS-K:2011 Kindergarten–First Grade Data File and Electronic Codebook, Public Version (NCES 2015–078). U.S. Department of Education. National Center for Education Statistics.

  • Ursache, A., Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). The promotion of self-regulation as a means of enhancing school readiness and early achievement in children at risk for school failure. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 122–128. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00209.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P., & Espin, C. A. (2012). Connecting cognitive theory and assessment: Measuring individual differences in reading comprehension. School Psychology Review, 41(3), 315–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Sluis, S., De Jong, P. F., & Van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35, 427–449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, A., & Kieffer, M. J. (2021). Exploring the English language comprehension, reading fluency, and executive functions of Spanish-English bilingual adolescents with reading difficulties. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 38(3), 233–252. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2021.1943580

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S. S., Heaton, R. K., Tulsky, D. S., Zelazo, P. D., Bauer, P. J., ... & Gershon, R. C. (2013). Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80(11 Supplement 3), S54-S64.

  • Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., Blair, C., Bierman, K. L., & Nelson, K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 43–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016738

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xue, Y., & Meisels, S. J. (2004). Early literacy instruction and learning in kindergarten: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten Class of 1998–1999. American Educational Research Journal, 41(1), 191–229. https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312041001191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pieper, S. (2013). Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in children: A meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1, 297–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by 2 P20 HD075443 and Core Grant HD15052 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development to Vanderbilt University. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development or the National Institutes of Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jackie Eunjung Relyea.

Additional information

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Relyea, J.E., Cho, E. & Zagata, E. First-grade multilingual students’ executive function profiles and links to English reading achievement and difficulties: a person-centered latent profile analysis. Ann. of Dyslexia 73, 29–52 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-022-00272-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-022-00272-2

Keywords

Navigation