Skip to main content
Log in

Subextraction in Japanese and subject-object symmetry

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

For a long time Japanese was taken to be a language lacking a subject-object asymmetry in subextraction. Two recent experimental studies have challenged this conclusion suggesting that Japanese complex NP (CNP) subjects are more opaque to subextraction than CNP objects (Jurka 2010; Jurka et al. 2011). Given the significance of this claim for the cross-linguistic landscape of subextraction phenomena, this study further explores the nature of subextraction phenomena in Japanese with three acceptability judgment experiments. We show that overt subextraction (scrambling) out of CNP subjects and objects results in similar acceptability ratings once the relative weight and order of constituents are properly controlled for. Recent experimental work which suggested that Japanese has a subject-object asymmetry to subextraction predicted that wh-in-situ adjuncts should lead to greater degradation for subextraction out of subjects as compared to subextraction out of objects. To test this prediction, we also present novel experimental data on wh-in-situ phrases inside subjects and objects in Japanese. Our results show that the argument/adjunct status of wh-in-situ phrases does not interact with the subject/object status of the CNPs, further invalidating the recent claims. Together these findings support the traditional view that Japanese has no subject-object asymmetry in subextraction, whether overt or covert. Having restored the status of Japanese as a language with no subject-object asymmetry in subextraction, we discuss possible reasons that could account for the absence of such an asymmetry. We suggest that the lack of asymmetry is due to Japanese subjects’ inability to satisfy the EPP on T/C; we further contend that cross-linguistic variation in subject transparency follows from whether or not the feature D comes from subjects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Rackowski and Richards (2005) argue that only constituents that enter into an Agree relation with a phase head (complements) are transparent to subextraction. This follows from the assumption that once a probe has entered into an Agree relation with a particular goal, it can disregard that goal for future Agree relations, i.e., it can enter into an Agree relation with a constituent internal to the former goal. Under their proposal, subextraction out of subjects is not permitted because subjects do not enter an Agree relation with v. For languages that allow for subextraction out of subjects, Rackowski and Richards (2005) suggest that subjects actually enter into an Agree relation with v (585; fn. 15). Under Müller’s (2010) proposal, in order for a phrase to move to a phase edge, an edge feature must be assigned to the phase head. Edge features can be assigned to a phase head freely as long as the head has an undischarged feature and therefore remains active in the derivation. Once a phase head has discharged all its features, it becomes inert and no edge feature can be assigned to it. This means that subextraction from a subject is blocked if the operation that merges a subject is the final operation taking place in a phase. Subextraction out of a moved phrase is blocked for the same reason.

  2. Tamaoka et al. (2003) examined the effects of relative weight and order of constituents in processing and comprehension of Japanese sentences. Their results reveal no effects of constituent weight or order on either reading time or comprehension accuracy of Japanese transitive sentences, both at phrase- and sentence-levels.

  3. We manipulated the relative order and length of constituents within the embedded clause since the CNP is always inside the embedded clause. While previous studies report the effects of the long-before-short preference in simple sentences (Dryer 1980; Hawkins 1994; Yamashita and Chang 2001; Yamashita 2002), we expected that the effects of the preference would be seen in embedded clauses as well.

  4. Ross (1967, 1986) argues that Japanese CNPs with a complement clause headed by koto ‘fact’ are not islands (i) while CNPs with a complement clause headed by to yuu, literally ‘that say,’ are (ii), based on the observation that relativization out of CNPs results in unacceptability only with CNPs with to yuu, as in (ii-b):

    1. (i)
      figure i
    1. (ii)
      figure j

    Haig (1976), on the other hand, argues that scrambling can occur from a CNP with a complement clause headed by by to yuu (iii-a), while scrambling out of a relative clause (RC) is impossible (iii-b).

    1. (iii)
      figure k

    Finally, Saito (1985) sides with Haig (1976) in that scrambling out of a RC (iv-a) is worse than that out of a CNP with a complement clause headed by to yuu (iv-b). Nevertheless, Saito considers both cases to instantiate island violations. Saito further notes that subextraction out of a CNP with a complement clause headed by to yuu is much more degraded if the CNP is definite, for instance, accompanied by a demonstrative.

    1. (iv)
      figure l

    Thus, a consensus in the theoretical literature appears to be that Japanese RCs are islands whereas the island status of the CNPs with koto ‘fact’ and to yuu ‘that say’ has remained controversial. More recently, Fukuda and Sprouse (2017) used a factorial design to systematically compare subextraction out of a CP complement with subextraction out of different structures including RCs and CNPs headed by to yuu. Their study found a significant interaction between the structure type (CNP vs. RC) and subextraction (no scrambling vs. scrambling), supporting the claim that RCs are islands in Japanese. The comparison between CPs and CNPs with to yuu, however, found no significant interaction between the structure type (CPs vs. CNPs with to yuu) and subextraction (no scrambling vs. scrambling), suggesting that CNPs with to yuu are not islands. Taken together with the observation that CNPs headed by koto are more transparent to subextraction than CNPs headed by to yuu, Fukuda and Sprouse’s (2017) results suggest that it is safe to assume that CNPs headed by koto are not islands.

  5. The results below will show that the baseline acceptability is actually much higher for A sentences than for B sentences, which indicates that the processing cost of CNP arguments is negligible.

  6. But see below and also Sect. 7 for further discussions of this effect.

  7. In Table 3, CNPS stands for “CNP subject.”

  8. CNPO stands for “CNP object.”

  9. A reviewer points out that the spell-out/phase constraint and the freezing constraint may be operating simultaneously; that much can be deduced from the findings reported in Jurka (2010) and Jurka et al. (2011) for German. Under such a scenario, 1A violates both of the constraints (i.e., subextraction out of a moved external argument) while 3A only violates the freezing constraint (i.e., subextraction out of a moved internal argument). We did not include our prediction under such a scenario in the table because it is not clear if and how acceptability judgments reflect simultaneous violations of multiple constraints: should we expect an additive effect? Our results in Experiment 1 and 2 show, however, that in Japanese neither the freezing effect nor the spell-out/phase effect is observed. Thus, we can conclude that a scenario where both effects are present is equally untenable.

  10. Again, under the scenario in which both the positional constraint and the freezing constraint are simultaneously operating, (i) violates both of the constrains (i.e., subextraction out of a moved external argument) while (ii) only violates the positional constraint (i.e., subextraction out of a moved internal argument). As stated in fn. 10, we did not discuss the possibility of both constraints operating. First, it is not clear if and how acceptability judgments reflect the difference between a single violation of a constraint and simultaneous multiple violations of multiple constraints, and second, Experiments 1 and 2 show that neither the freezing effect nor the positional effect is observed with Japanese arguments.

  11. The translation of this adjunct may be somewhat misleading, suggesting that the relevant PP is a parenthetical; however, these constituents in our stimuli are bona fide PPs and can be used as a signpost for short scrambling.

  12. Here one may worry that the sentence-initial PP X-niyoruto ‘according to X’ may be interpreted as a part of the following CNP. Under such parsing of the experimental sentences, the scrambling could be analyzed as taking place inside the CNP, contrary to our intention. However, we believe that the suggested parsing of the PP as a constituent of the CNP is unlikely for several reasons. First, the experimental sentences were presented with punctuation which facilitates the intended parse. In particular, a comma was placed between the PP and the initial element of the CNP in all the experimental sentences, discouraging the parsing of the PP together with the following CNP. Second, all the experimental sentences involved X-niyoruto ‘according to X’ and an evidential marker such as yooda ‘seem’ in the matrix predicate. This is a very common pattern, and the presence of an evidential marker in the matrix predicate strongly suggests that the PP also belongs to the matrix clause. Finally, the CNPs in the experimental sentences all involved -koto ‘fact’, which triggers a presupposition that the proposition expressed by a -koto clause is true (e.g., Kuno 1973). In contrast, the PP Xniyoruto ‘according to X’ explicitly denies such a presupposition. This semantic mismatch between -koto and X-niyoruto makes the hypothesized constituent with the PP the CNP headed by -koto infelicitous. Thus, we believe that speakers are unlikely to have entertained a parse of X-niyoruro according to X as a part of a CNP headed by -koto.

  13. These fillers are different from the fillers used in Experiment 1.

  14. An anonymous reviewer questions whether null results can be taken as evidence for the null hypothesis (i.e., that there is no meaningful difference between two populations). Sprouse and Almeida (2017) provide a useful guideline for the minimum number of participants that acceptability judgment studies with different rating methods need in order to have sufficient statistical power for null results to be taken as evidence for the null hypothesis (80% or above). Their estimates indicate that, for a study that examines a medium-effect size acceptability contrast with a Likert scale, the minimum number of participants required to reach the 80% power is 37 under a conservative assumption that each participant rated only one item per experimental condition (Sprouse and Almeida 2017: 21, Fig. 3). Since the numbers of participants for the three experiments whose results are reported in this study are 62, 43 and 41, respectively, we contend that the null results that we obtained constitute evidence for the null hypothesis.

  15. Why also exhibits unusual behavior in languages with overt wh-movement, for example, in Italian (e.g., Rizzi 1990; Ko 2005).

  16. These fillers are different from the fillers used in Experiments 1 and 2.

  17. Recall the argument in fn. 5 that Japanese CNPs headed by -koto ‘fact’ and by to yuu ‘that say’ are not islands. Given this claim, the degraded nature of ‘why’-sentences in Experiment 3 cannot be attributed merely to island-sensitivity; even embedding ‘why’ inside a non-island causes degradation. For future research, we would like to see whether naze ‘why’ becomes degraded even inside a simple embedded clause.

    A ban on long-distance interpretations of why is also observed in English sluicing (although it is well-known that English sluicing is island-insensitive). Lasnik (2005) notes that the sluiced wh-phrase why in examples such as (i) only allows the ‘short distance’ reading in (b), suggesting that such why obeys a stricter condition than island effects.

    1. (i)
      figure y

    In sum, although naze ‘why’ inside a non-island CNP is degraded, there is no additional island effect for a subject CNP.

  18. Sprouse et al. (2015) found no interaction between argument types and extraction types with Italian relative clauses, while the interaction was significant with English relative clauses. This is compatible with the claim by Rizzi (1982) that subjects are not islands in Italian relative clauses, and Sprouse et al. argue that the dependencies in wh-questions and relative clauses cannot be assumed to be identical. While this conclusion potentially impacts the generality of subject island effects, the potential uniqueness of relativization is irrelevant to the current experiments that focus on scrambling and covert wh-movement.

  19. In German, Jurka (2010) and Jurka et al. (2011) report that both the external/internal distinction and the moved/in situ distinction have a significant impact on the acceptability of subextraction out of arguments.

  20. The proposed distinction between Italian and Japanese is tentative, as we only examined subextraction out of moved vs. in situ unaccusative subjects in Japanese with Experiment 1, and we do not have data on subextraction out of moved and in situ external arguments (i.e. transitive and unergative subjects) in Japanese.

  21. Following Chomsky (2000, 2001), some freezing-based accounts restrict freezing to movement for case and/or agreement, on the assumption that criterial properties such as [focus] or [topic] do not influence minimality considerations (cf. Gallego and Uriagereka 2008). On this particular approach to freezing, the transparency of Japanese subjects may be taken to follow from the absence of phi-feature agreement (Kuroda 1985, 1988; Fukui 1986, 1995, 2006).

  22. We would like to underscore that in Jurka’s model, the subject condition that cannot be reduced to freezing effects is universal. That is why the results obtained by Jurka and colleagues for Japanese were of such importance.

  23. There are four genders in the singular and two in the plural (indicated as (n)IPL in the glosses below). The verbal exponent of agreement is always a prefix, although agreement is marked only on a subset of vowel-initial verbs (Polinsky and Potsdam 2001; Polinsky 2003).

  24. With respect to non-agreeing subjects in Hindi, it has been claimed that they satisfy the EPP on T but that their movement to T is EPP-driven, not Agree-driven (Anand and Nevins 2006); evidence for that comes from the lack of reconstruction for scope, along the arguments developed by Miyagawa (2001) for Japanese. Hindi ergative expressions are islands (Alok 2016), but their island status may be independently motivated by their status as PPs, not DPs, with the ergative exponent ne being a postposition (see Mahajan 1997 for arguments supporting this analysis).

  25. There are two main proposals concerning the EPP on T in Russian. According to Bailyn (2004, 2012), a designated A-position in [Spec, T] can host a variety of arguments. According to Slioussar (2011) and Krejci et al. (2018), only agreed-with arguments appear in that position, whereas all other arguments are in a different, higher, A-bar position.

  26. We thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing out these potential problems.

References

  • Agbayani, Brian, Chris Golston, and Toru Ishii. 2015. Syntactic and prosodic scrambling in Japanese. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 33: 47–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aissen, Judith. 1996. Pied-piping, abstract agreement, and functional projections in Tzotzil. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 14: 447–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akatsuka, Noriko. 1976. Reflexivization: A transformational approach. In Syntax and semantics 6: Japanese generative grammar, ed. Masayoshi Shibatani, 51–116. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexiadou, Artemis, and Elena Anagnostopoulou. 1998. Parametrizing AGR: Word order, v-movement and EPP-checking. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 16: 491–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alok, Depak. 2016. The syntax of a split: The case of Hindi and Magahi. Paper presented at RULingXI, Rutgers University.

  • Anand, Pranav, and Andrew Nevins. 2006. The locus of ergative case assignment: Evidence from scope. In Ergativity: Emerging issues, eds. Alana Johns, Diane Massam, and Juvenal Ndayiragije, 3–26. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aoun, Joseph, and Y.-H. Audrey Li. 1993. Wh-elements in situ: Syntax or LF? Linguistic Inquiry 24: 199–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailyn, John. 2004. Generalized inversion. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 22: 1–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailyn, John. 2012. The syntax of Russian. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, Dale. J., Roger Levy, Christoph Scheepers, and Harry J. Tily. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68: 255–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belletti, Adriana, and Luigi Rizzi. 1988. Psych-verbs and theta theory. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 6: 291–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benmamoun, Elabbas. 2000. The feature structure of functional categories: A comparative study of Arabic dialects. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobaljik, Jonathan D., and Kazuko Yatsushiro. 2006. Problems with honorification-as-agreement in Japanese: A reply to Boeckx and Niinuma. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 24: 355–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, Cedric. 2008. Understanding minimalist syntax: Lessons from locality in long-distance dependencies. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, Cedric, and Fumikazu Niinuma. 2004. Conditions on agreement in Japanese. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 22: 453–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bošković, Željko. 2005. On the locality of left branch extraction and the structure of NP. Studia Linguistica 59: 1–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bošković, Željko. 2008. What will you have, DP or NP? In North East Linguistic Society (NELS) 37, eds. Emily Elfner and Martin Walkow. 101–114. Amherst: University of Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bošković, Željko. 2009. More on the No-DP analysis of article-less languages. Studia Linguistica 63: 187–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1973. Conditions on transformations. In A Festschrtft for Morris Halle, eds. Stephen R. Anderson and Paul Kiparsky, 232–286. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist program. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: the framework. In Step by step: Essays on Minimalist syntax in honor of Hward Lasnik, eds. Roger Martin, David Michaels, and Juan Uriagereka, 890–155. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Ken Hale: A life in language, ed. Michael Kenstowicz, 1–52. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, Sandra. 1991. Sentential subjects and proper binding in Chamorro. In Interdisciplinary approaches to language: Essays in honor of S.-Y. Kuroda, eds. Carol Georgopoulos and Roberta Ishihara, 75–99. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, Sandra. 1998. The design of agreement. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coon, Jessica. 2009. Interrogative possessors and the problem with pied-piping in Chol. Linguistic Inquiry 40: 165–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corver, Norbert. 1990. The syntax of left branch extractions. PhD diss., Tilburg University.

  • Corver, Norbert. 1992. On deriving certain left branch extraction asymmetries: A case study in parametric syntax. In Northeast Linguistic Society (NELS) 22, 67–84. Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corver, Norbert. 2017. Subextraction. In Wiley Blackwell companion to syntax, 2nd edn., eds. Martin Everaert and Henk C. Van Riemsdijk, 566–600. Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118358733.wbsyncom054.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cowart, Wayne. 1997. Experimental syntax: Applying objective methods to sentence judgments. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, William D., and Stanley Dubinsky. 2001. Functional architecture and the distribution of subject properties. In Objects and other subjects: Grammatical functions, functional categories and configurationality, eds. William D. Davies and Stanley Dubinsky, 247–279. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dryer, Matthew S. 1980. The positional tendencies of sentential noun phrases in universal grammar. The Canadian Journal of Linguistics 25: 123–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • É. Kiss, Katalin. 1995. NP-movement, operator movement, and scrambling in Hungarian. In Discourse configurational languages, ed. Katalin É. Kiss, 207–243. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fukuda, Shin and Jon Sprouse. 2017. Are there islands in Japanese? Ms., University of Hawai‘i at Mānoa and University of Connecticut.

  • Fukui, Naoki. 1986. A theory of category projection and its applications. PhD diss., MIT.

  • Fukui, Naoki. 1988. LF extraction of naze: Some theoretical implications. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 6: 503–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fukui, Naoki. 1995. Theory of projection in syntax. Stanford and Tokyo: CLSI and Kuroshio.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fukui, Naoki. 2006. Theoretical comparative syntax: Studies in macroparameters. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallego, Angel, and Juan Uriagereka. 2008. Freezing effects. 18th Colloquium on Generative Grammar, Lisbon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerdts, Donna. 1988. Object and absolutive in Halkomelem Salish. New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haig, John H. 1976. Shadow pronoun deletion in Japanese. Linguistic Inquiry 7: 363–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harder, Peter. 1996. Functional semantics: A theory of meaning, structure, and tense in English. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, John A. 1994. A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoji, Hajime. 1985. Logical form constraints and the configurational structures in Japanese. PhD diss., University of Washington.

  • Huang, C.-T. James. 1982. Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar. PhD diss., MIT.

  • Ishii, Toru. 1997. An asymmetry in the composition of phrase structure and its consequences. PhD diss., University of California, Irvine.

  • Iwata, Seizi. 1995. The distinctive character of psych-verbs as causatives. Linguistic Analysis 1–2: 95–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, Kyle. 2003. Towards an etiology of adjunct islands. Nordlyd 31(1): 187–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jurka, Johannes. 2010. The importance of being a complement: The CED effects revisited. PhD diss., University of Maryland.

  • Jurka, Johannes, Chizuru Nakao, and Akira Omaki. 2011. It’s not the end of the CED as we know it: Revisiting German and Japanese subject islands. In West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (WCCFL) 28, eds. Mary Byram Washburn, Katherine McKinney-Bock, Erika Varis, Ann Sawyer, and Barbara Tomaszewicz. 124–132. Somerville: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kikuchi, Akira. 1987. Comparative deletion in Japanese. Ms., Yamagata University.

  • Kishimoto, Hideki. 2005. Wh-in-situ and movement in Sinhara questions. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 23: 1–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kishimoto, Hideki. 2006. Japanese syntactic nominalization and VP-internal syntax. Lingua 116: 771–810.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitagawa, Yoshihisa. 1986. Subjects in Japanese and English. PhD diss., University of Massachusetts.

  • Ko, Heejeong. 2005. Syntax of why-in-situ: Merge into [Spec, CP] in the overt syntax. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 23: 867–916.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kravtchenko, Ekaterina, Maria Polinsky, and Ming Xiang. 2009. Are all subject islands created equal? Poster presented at CUNY, UC Davis.

  • Krejci, Bonnie, Vera Gribanova, and Boris Harizanov. 2018. AGREE-dependent A-movement and low copy pronunciation in Russian. In Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics (FASL) 25: The Third Cornell Meeting 2016, eds. Wayles Browne et al., 119–139. Ann Arbor: Michigan Slavic Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuno, Susumu. 1973. The structure of the Japanese language. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, Alexandra, Per B. Brockhoff, and Rune H. B. Christensen. 2016. lmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models. R package version 2.0-30. Available at https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/lmerTest.pdf.

  • Kuroda, S.-Y. 1980. Bun no Kozo [The structure of sentences]. In Nichi-ei Hikaku Koza II: Bunpo, ed. Tetsuya Kunihiro, 23–62. Tokyo: Taishukan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuroda, S.-Y. 1985. Whether we agree or not: Rough ideas about the comparative syntax of English and Japanese. Ms., UCSD.

  • Kuroda, S.-Y. 1988. Whether we agree or not: A comparative syntax of English and Japanese. Linguisticae Investigationes 12: 1–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, Idan. 2007. EPP extensions. Linguistic Inquiry 38: 485–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, Idan. 2010. The locative syntax of experiencers. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasnik, Howard. 2005. How to evade moving violations. Handout from a lecture at LSA Summer Institute, MIT.

  • Lasnik, Howard, and Myung-Kwan Park. 2003. The EPP and the subject condition under sluicing. Linguistic Inquiry 34: 649–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasnik, Howard, and Mamoru Saito. 1992. Moveα: Conditions on its application and output. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohndal, Terje. 2011. Freezing effects and objects. Journal of Linguistics 47: 163–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longobardi, Giuseppe. 1994. Reference and proper names. Linguistic Inquiry 25: 609–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longobardi, Giuseppe. 2005. Toward a unified grammar of reference. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 24: 5–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longobardi, Giuseppe. 2008. Reference to individuals, person, and the variety of mapping parameters. In Essays on nominal determination: From morphology to discourse management, eds. Henrik Høeg Müller and Alex Klinge, 189–211. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahajan, Anoup. 1997. Universal grammar and the typology of ergative languages. In Studies on universal grammar and typological variation, eds. Artemis Alexiadou and Thomas A. Hall, 35–57. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marušič, Franc, and Rok Žaucer. 2010. Clitic doubling in a determinerless language with second position clitics. In Formal Studies in Slavic Linguistics, Proceedings of FDSL 7.5, eds. Gerhild Zybatow, Philip Dudchuk, Serge Minor, and Ekaterina Pshehotskaya, 101–115. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumura, Hiromi. 1996. On Japanese psych-verbs. In Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 15, eds. Päivi Koskinen and Carolyn Smallwood, 123–140. University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, James. 1996. Subjects and subject positions in Irish. In The syntax of Celtic languages: A comparative perspective, eds. Robert Borsley and Ian Roberts, 241–283. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, Thomas, and Sandya Sundaresan. 2018. What the EPP and comp-trace effects have in common: Constraining silent elements at the edge. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 3(1): 43. doi:http://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyagawa, Shigeru. 2001. The EPP, scrambling and wh-in-situ. In Ken Hale: A life in language, ed. Michael Kenstowicz. 293–331. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyagawa, Shigeru. 2010. Why agree? Why move?: Unifying agreement-based and discourse-configurational languages. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, Gereon. 2010. On deriving CED effects from the PIC. Linguistic Inquiry 41: 35–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishigauchi, Taisuke. 1990. Quantification in the theory of grammar. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuñes, Jairo, and Juan Uriagereka. 2000. Cyclicity and extraction domains. Syntax 3: 20–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partee, Barbara H. 1973. Some structural analogies between tenses and pronouns in English. The Journal of Philosophy 70: 601–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pesetsky, David. 1995. Zero syntax: Experiencers and cascades. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polinsky, Maria. 2003. Non-canonical agreement is canonical. Transactions of the Philological Society 101: 279–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polinsky, Maria. 2018. Freezing and phi-feature agreement: On the role of [PERSON]. In Freezing: Theoretical approaches and empirical domains, eds. Jutta M. Hartmann et al., 284–316. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polinsky, Maria, Carlos Gómez Gallo, Peter Graff, Ekaterina Kravtchenko, Adam M. Morgan, and Anne Sturgeon. 2013. Subject islands are different. In Experimental syntax and island effects, eds. Jon Sprouse and Norbert Hornstein, 286–309. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polinsky, Maria, and Eric Potsdam. 2001. Long-distance agreement and topic in Tsez. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 19: 583–646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rackowski, Andrea, and Norvin Richards. 2005. Phase edge and extraction: A Tagalog case study. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 565–599.

    Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at https://www.R-project.org/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, Norvin. 1997. What moves where when in which languages? PhD diss., MIT.

  • Richards, Norvin. 2000. An island effect in Japanese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 9: 187–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, Norvin. 2008. Wh-questions. In The Oxford handbook of Japanese linguistics, eds. Shigeru Miyagawa and Mamoru Saito, 348–371. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, Norvin. 2016. Contiguity theory. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 1982. Issues in Italian syntax. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 1990. Relativized minimality. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 2006. On the form of chains: Criterial positions and ECP Effects. In WH-movement: Moving on, eds. Lisa Cheng and Norbert Corver, 97–134. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 2010. On some properties of criterial freezing. In The complementizer phase: Subjects and operators, 17–32. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi, and Ur Shlonsky. 2007. Strategies of subject extraction. In Interfaces + Recursion = Language?: Chomsky’s minimalism and the view from syntax-semantics, eds. Uli Sauerland and Hans-Martin Gärtner, 115–160. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, Ian. 2010. Agreement and head movement: Clitics, incorporation, and defective goals. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, John R. 1967. Constraints on variables in syntax. PhD diss., MIT

  • Ross, John R. 1986. Infinite syntax! Norwood: ABLEX.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothstein, Susan. 2001. Predicates and their subjects. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saito, Mamoru. 1983. Case and government in Japanese. In West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (WCCFL) 2, eds. Michael Barlow, Daniel P. Flickinger, and Nancy Wiegand. 247–259. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saito, Mamoru. 1985. Some asymmetries in Japanese and their theoretical implications. PhD diss., MIT

  • Saito, Mamoru. 1992. Long distance scrambling in Japanese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 1: 69–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saito, Mamoru, and Hajime Hoji. 1983. Weak crossover and Move α in Japanese. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 1: 245–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saito, Mamoru, T.-H. Jonah Lin, and Keiko Murasugi. 2008. N’-ellipsis and the structure of noun phrases in Chinese and Japanese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 17: 247–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schütze, Carson T., and Jon Sprouse. 2013. Judgment data. In Research methods in linguistics, eds. Rob J. Podesva and Devyani Sharma, 27–50. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimoyama, Junko. 2001. Wh-costructions in Japanese. PhD diss., University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

  • Slioussar, Natalia. 2011. Russian and the EPP requirement in the Tense domain. Lingua 121: 2048–2068.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smeets, Rieks. 1984. Studies in West Circassian phonology and morphology. Leiden: Hakuchi Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soltan, Usama. 2007. On postcyclic Merge and Agree in syntactic derivations: First conjunct agreement in Standard Arabic revisited. In Perspectives on Arabic linguistics, ed. Elabbas Benmamoun. Vol. 19, 191–213. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soltan, Usama. 2011. On issues of Arabic syntax: An essay in syntactic argumentation. Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 3: 236–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprouse, Jon, and Diogo Almeida. 2017. Design sensitivity and statistical power in acceptability judgment experiments. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2(1)14: 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprouse, Jon, Shin Fukuda, Hajime Ono, and Robert Kluender. 2011. Reverse island effects and the backward search for a licensor in multiple wh-questions. Syntax 14: 179–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprouse, Jon, Ivano Caponigro, Ciro Greco, and Carlo Cecchetto. 2015. Experimental syntax and the variation of island effects in English and Italian. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 34: 307–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stepanov, Arthur. 2007. The end of the CED? Minimalism and extraction domains. Syntax 10: 80–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, Tatsuya. 1989. A syntactic analysis of an honorific construction o-ni naru under the DP hypothesis: Towards a unified theory of honorification. In West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (WCCFL) 8, eds. E. Jane Fee and Katherine Hunt. 384–398. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, Daiko. 1994. Minimality of movement. PhD diss., University of Connecticut.

  • Tamaoka, Katsuo, Hiromu Sakai, Jun-ichiro Kawahara, and Yayoi Miyaoka. 2003. The effects of phrase-length order and scrambling in the processing of visually presented Japanese sentences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 32: 431–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Testelets, Jakov, ed. 2009. Aspekty polisintetizma. Očerki po grammatike adygejskogo jazyka. Moscow: RGGU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toribio, Almeida Jacqueline. 1990. Specifier-head agreement in Japanese. In West Coast Conference on Linguistics (WCCFL) 9, ed. Aaron Halpern. 535–548. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, W.-T. Dylan. 1994. On nominal islands and LE extraction in Chinese. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 12: 121–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ueda, Yukiko. 2008. Person restrictions and syntactic structure of Japanese modals. Scientific Approaches to Language 7: 123–150. Center for Language Sciences, Kanda University of International Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ueda, Yukiko. 2009. The right periphery in the Japanese CP. Scientific Approaches to Language 8: 95–118. Tokyo: Center for Language Sciences, Kanda University of International Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uriagereka, Juan. 1999. Multiple Spell-Out. In Working minimalism, eds. Samuel D. Epstein and Norbert Hornstein, 251–282. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, Akira. 1992. Subjacency and s-structure movement of wh-in-situ. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 1: 255–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, Akira. 2006. Functional projections of nominals in Japanese: Syntax of classifiers. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 24: 241–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wexler, Kenneth, and Peter Culicover. 1981. Formal principles of language acquisition. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wurmbrand, Susi. 2006. Licensing case. Journal of Germanic Linguistics 18: 175–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamashita, Hiroko. 2002. Scrambled sentences in Japanese: Linguistic properties and motivations for production. Text 22: 597–633.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamashita, Hiroko, and Franklin Chang. 2001. Long-before-short preference in the production of a head final language. Cognition 81: B45–B55.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Ken Hiraiwa, Hideki Maki, and Hajime Ono, for allowing us to run our pilot studies and experiments with their students, and Ted Levin, Kamil Deen, and Julie Jiang for reading earlier drafts of this paper and providing us with valuable comments and suggestions. We are grateful to three anonymous reviewers, Caroline Heycock, and Julie Legate for their helpful suggestions and comments that greatly improved the content, organization and presentation of this paper. Many thanks are also due to the audiences at the 10th Workshop on Altaic Formal Linguistics (WAFL10) and the 8th Formal Approaches to Japanese Linguistics (FAJL8), where earlier versions of this study were presented. This work is an output of a research project implemented as part of the Basic Research Program at the National Research University Higher School of Economics (HSE). It was also supported in part by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP17K02823 to Chizuru Nakao and NSF grant BCS-1619857 to Maria Polinsky.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shin Fukuda.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

The first author of this paper, Akira Omaki, passed away on August 6, 2018, following complications from lymphoma. Akira worked on this project while fighting cancer, until early June 2018, when he had to put his research activities on hold to focus on a new treatment. We feel honored and privileged to have been able to share this piece of research, one of Akira’s last, with him. We hope that those who knew Akira can see his passion, dedication, and professionalism in this paper.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below are the links to the electronic supplementary material.

(PDF 427 kB)

(PDF 176 kB)

(PDF 163 kB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Omaki, A., Fukuda, S., Nakao, C. et al. Subextraction in Japanese and subject-object symmetry. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 38, 627–669 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-019-09449-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-019-09449-8

Keywords

Navigation