Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Developmental Trajectories of Executive Functioning and Puberty in Boys and Girls

  • Empirical Research
  • Published:
Journal of Youth and Adolescence Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There are substantial changes in executive functioning during adolescence that may correspond with the onset and progression of puberty. The current study examines associations between pubertal development (timing and tempo) and changes in specific executive functioning skills (i.e., attention and self-control) across the transition from childhood to adolescence (ages 9.5–15.5) using data from the Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (1099 youth; 52% female, 81% White, 83% above the poverty line). The findings indicated that early maturation was associated with faster increases in attention skills over adolescence for both boys and girls. Further, early maturation predicted worse self-control among girls but not boys. This study provides new insights on executive functioning during the transition to adolescence—a period of both vulnerability and opportunity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Data and Sharing Declaration

The data that support the findings of this study are available from Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study, and so are not publicly available. However, data are available from the authors upon reasonable request and with permission of Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research.

References

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1991). Child behavior checklist. Burlington VT: University of Vermont. Department of psychiatry.

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Ruffle, T. M. (2000). The Child Behavior Checklist and related forms for assessing behavioral/emotional problems and competencies. Pediatrics in review, 21(8), 265–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, V. A., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R., & Catroppa, C. (2001). Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an australian sample. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 385–406. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN2001_5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arain, M., Haque, M., Johal, L., Mathur, P., Nel, W., Rais, A., & Sharma, S. (2013). Maturation of the adolescent brain. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 9, 449–461. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S39776.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Arim, R. G., Tramonte, L., Shapka, J. D., Dahinten, V. S., & Willms, J. D. (2011). The family antecedents and the subsequent outcomes of early puberty. Journal of Youth Adolescent, 40(11), 1423–1435. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9638-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baams, L., Dubas, J. S., Overbeek, G., & Van Aken, M. A. (2015). Transitions in body and behavior: a meta-analytic study on the relationship between pubertal development and adolescent sexual behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health, 56(6), 586–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bearman, S. K., Presnell, K., Martinez, E., & Stice, E. (2006). The skinny on body dissatisfaction: a longitudinal study of adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35(2), 217–229.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 327–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Biro, F. M., Pinney, S. M., Huang, B., Baker, E. R., Walt Chandler, D., & Dorn, L. D. (2014). Hormone changes in peripubertal girls. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 99(10), 3829-3835. Blakemore, S. J. (2008). Development of the social brain during adolescence. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 40–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701508715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S. J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3-4), 296–312. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bleil, M. E., Booth-LaForce, C., & Benner, A. D. (2017). Race disparities in pubertal timing: implications for cardiovascular disease risk among african american women. Population Research and Policy Review, 36(5), 717–738.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brix, N., Ernst, A., Lauridsen, L. L. B., Parner, E., Stovring, H., Olsen, J., & Ramlau-Hansen, C. H. (2018). Timing of puberty in boys and girls: a population-based study. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 33(1), 70–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/ppe.12507.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bub, K. L., McCartney, K., & Willett, J. B. (2007). Behavior problem trajectories and first-grade cognitive ability and achievement skills: a latent growth curve analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 653–670. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28(1), 62–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castellanos-Ryan, N., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Séguin, J. R. (2013). Pubertal development, personality, and substance use: a 10-year longitudinal study from childhood to adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(3), 782.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chumlea, W. C., Schubert, C. M., Roche, A. F., Kulin, H. E., Lee, P. A., Himes, J. H., & Sun, S. S. (2003). Age at menarche and racial comparisons in us girls. Pediatrics, 111(1), 110–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cottrell, J. M., Newman, D. A., & Roisman, G. I. (2015). Explaining the black-white gap in cognitive test scores: toward a theory of adverse impact. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(6), 1713–1736. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deardorff, J., Hayward, C., Wilson, K. A., Bryson, S., Hammer, L. D., & Agras, S. (2007). Puberty and gender interact to predict social anxiety symptoms in early adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41(1), 102–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deardorff, J., Hoyt, L. T., Carter, R., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2018). Next steps in puberty research: broadening the lens toward understudied populations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29, 133-154.

  • Ferrer, E., Balluerka, N., & Widaman, K. F. (2008). Factorial invariance and the specification of second-order latent growth models. Methodology (Gott), 4(1), 22–36. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-2241.4.1.22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ge, X., & Natsuaki, M. N. (2009). In search of explanations for early pubteral timing effects on developmental psychopathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(6), 327–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goddings, A.-L., Mills, K. L., Clasen, L. S., Giedd, J. N., Viner, R. M., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2014). The influence of puberty on subcortical brain development. NeuroImage, 88, 242–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gresham, F. M., & Elliot, S. N. (1990). Manual for the social skills rating system. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

  • Grimm, K. J., Pianta, R. C., & Konold, T. (2009). Longitudinal multitrait-multimethod models for developmental research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 44(2), 233–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gur, R. C., Richard, J., Calkins, M. E., Chiavacci, R., Hansen, J. A., Bilker, W. B., & Abou-Sleiman, P. M. (2012). Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8− 21. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 251.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hedges, K., & Korchmaros, J. D. (2016). Pubertal timing and substance abuse treatment outcomes: an analysis of early menarche on substance use patterns. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 25(6), 598–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, A. C., Ramirez, A. M., Lee, G., North, S. J., Askari, M. S., West, R. L., & Sommer, M. (2016). Puberty experiences of low-income girls in the united states: a systematic review of qualitative literature from 2000 to 2014. Journal of Adolescent Health, 60(4), 363–379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.10.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, C., Brieant, A., Kahn, R., Deater-Deckard, K., & Kim-Spoon, J. (2018). Structural home environment effects on developmental trajectories of self-control and adolescent risk taking. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, C. J., Kim-Spoon, J., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2016). Linking executive function and peer problems from early childhood through middle adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(1), 31–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 617–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Icenogle, G., Steinberg, L., Olino, T. M., Shulman, E. P., Chein, J., Alampay, L. P., & Uribe Tirado, L. M. (2017). Puberty predicts approach but not avoidance on the iowa gambling task in a multinational sample. Child Development, 88(5), 1598–1614. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2011). Dynamic changes in anger, externalizing and internalizing problems: attention and regulation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(2), 156–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koerselman, K., & Pekkarinen, T. (2017). The timing of puberty and gender differences in educational achievement IZA Discussion Papers 10889. Germany: IZA Institute of Labor Economics.

  • Koolschijn, P. C., Peper, J. S., & Crone, E. A. (2014). The influence of sex steroids on structural brain maturation in adolescence. PLoS ONE, 9(1), e83929 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0083929.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, G. M., & Farah, M. J. (2017). Executive function as a mediator between ses and academic achievement throughout childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 41(1), 94–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leon-Carrion, J., García-Orza, J., & Pérez-Santamaría, F. J. (2004). Development of the inhibitory component of the executive functions in children and adolescents. International Journal of Neuroscience, 114(10), 1291–1311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marceau, K., Ram, N., Houts, R. M., Grimm, K. J., & Susman, E. J. (2011). Individual differences in boys’ and girls’ timing and tempo of puberty: Modeling development with nonlinear growth models. Developmental psychology, 47(5), 1389.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mendle, J., Harden, K. P., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Graber, J. A. (2010). Development’s tortoise and hare: pubertal timing, pubertal tempo, and depressive symptoms in boys and girls. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1341–1353. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020205.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, J. (2015). A word on standardization in longitudinal studies: don’t. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1389.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, J., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., & Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(7), 2693–2698. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Muthen, B. (1983). Latent variable structural equation modeling with categorical data. Journal of Econometrics, 22, 43-65.

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998). Mplus user’s guide (Version 7). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén, 2004.

  • Nejati, V., Salehinejad, M. A., & Nitsche, M. A. (2018). Interaction of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (l-DLPFC) and right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in hot and cold executive functions: evidence from transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuroscience, 369, 109–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ng-Knight, T., Shelton, K. H., Riglin, L., McManus, I. C., Frederickson, N., & Rice, F. (2016). A longitudinal study of self-control at the transition to secondary school: considering the role of pubertal status and parenting. Journal of Adolescence, 50, 44–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2016). Annual research review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 361–383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12675.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noll, J. G., Trickett, P. K., Long, J. D., Negriff, S., Susman, E. J., Shalev, I., & Putnam, F. W. (2017). Childhood sexual abuse and early timing of puberty. Journal of Adolescent Health, 60(1), 65–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patton, G. C., & Viner, R. (2007). Pubertal transitions in health. The Lancet, 369(9567), 1130–1139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peper, J. S., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Crone, E. A., & van Honk, J. (2011). Sex steroids and brain structure in pubertal boys and girls: a mini-review of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience, 191, 28–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.02.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 621–637. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raffington, L., Prindle, J., Keresztes, A., Binder, J., Heim, C., & Shing, Y. L. (2018). Blunted cortisol stress reactivity in low–income children relates to lower memory function. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 90, 110–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raykov, T. (2000). On the large-sample bias, variance, and mean squared error of the conventional noncentrality parameter estimator of covariance structure models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 7(3), 431–441. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0703_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ribner, A. D., Willoughby, M. T., & Blair, C. B., Investigators, F. L. P. K. (2017). Executive function buffers the association between early math and later academic skills. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 869.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rogol, A. D., Clark, P. A., & Roemmich, J. N. (2000). Growth and pubertal development in children and adolescents: effects of diet and physical activity–. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 72(2), 521S–528S.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sabol, T. J., & Hoyt, L. T. (2017). The long arm of childhood: preschool associations with adolescent health. Developmental Psychology, 53(4), 752.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Satterthwaite, T. D., Shinohara, R. T., Wolf, D. H., Hopson, R. D., Elliott, M. A., Vandekar, S. N., & Gur, R. E. (2014). Impact of puberty on the evolution of cerebral perfusion during adolescence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(23), 8643–8648.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Seltzer, M. H., Frank, K. A., & Bryk, A. S. (1994). The metric matters: the sensitivity of conclusions about growth in student achievement to choice of metric. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 16(1), 41–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, E. P., Harden, K. P., Chein, J. M., & Steinberg, L. (2015). Sex differences in the developmental trajectories of impulse control and sensation-seeking from early adolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44(1), 1–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Squeglia, L. M., Jacobus, J., Sorg, S. F., Jernigan, T. L., & Tapert, S. F. (2013). Early adolescent cortical thinning is related to better neuropsychological performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(9), 962–970.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, L. (2010). A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Psychobiology, 52(3), 216–224. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsukayama, E., Toomey, S. L., Faith, M. S., & Duckworth, A. L. (2010). Self-control as a protective factor against overweight status in the transition from childhood to adolescence. JAMA Pediatrics, 164, 631–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walthall, J. C., Konold, T. R., & Pianta, R. C. (2005). Factor structure of the social skills rating system across child gender and ethnicity. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 23(3), 201–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman, A. M., Crockett, L. J., & Hoffman, L. (2017). Reward seeking and cognitive control: using the dual systems model to predict adolescent sexual behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27(4), 907–913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlfahrt-Veje, C., Korsholm Mouritsen, A., Hagen, C. P., Tinggaard, J., Grunnet Mieritz, M., Boas, M., & Main, K. M. (2016). Pubertal onset in boys and girls is influenced by pubertal timing of both parents. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 101, jc20161073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 354–360.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research uses data from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD SECCYD), a longitudinal, multi-site prospective project directed by a steering committee and funded through a series of cooperative agreements (U10s and a U01). We thank the principal investigators and families who participated in this study. Information on how to obtain the SECCYD data files is available on the NICHD SECCYD website (https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/icpsrweb/ICPSR/series/00233). No direct support was received from NICHD for this analysis and the views expressed in this article are the authors alone. The authors also gratefully acknowledge feedback from Dr. Terri Sabol (Northwestern University) and Dr. Ann Higgins (Fordham University) on early drafts of this manuscript.

Authors’ Contributions

NC and LTH conceived of the study. NC performed the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript; LTH oversaw and provided feedback on the statistical analysis and helped draft and edit the manuscript. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Natasha Chaku or Lindsay T. Hoyt.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All research reported on in the manuscript was conducted in compliance with APA ethical principles. The study consisted of secondary analyses of de-identified data, and therefore did not require formal consent or ethics board approval.

Additional information

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 4 and 5

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chaku, N., Hoyt, L.T. Developmental Trajectories of Executive Functioning and Puberty in Boys and Girls. J Youth Adolescence 48, 1365–1378 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01021-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01021-2

Navigation