Skip to main content

Consequences of Heuristic Distortions on SHM-Based Decision

  • Conference paper
  • First Online:
Civil Structural Health Monitoring (CSHM 2021)

Part of the book series: Lecture Notes in Civil Engineering ((LNCE,volume 156))

Included in the following conference series:

Abstract

The main purpose of structural health monitoring (SHM) is to provide accurate and real-time information about the state of a structure, which can be used as objective inputs for decision-making regarding its management. However, SHM and decision-making occur at various stages. SHM assesses the state of a structure based on the acquisition and interpretation of data, which is usually provided by sensors. Conversely, decision-making helps us to identify the optimal management action to undertake. Generally, the research community recognizes people tend to use irrational methods for their interpretation of monitoring data, instead of rational algorithms such as Bayesian inference. People use heuristics as efficient rules to simplify complex problems and overcome the limits in rationality and computation of the human brain. Even though the results are typically satisfactory, they can differ from those derived from a rational process. Many heuristic behaviors have been studied and demonstrated, with applications in various fields such as psychology, cognitive science, economics, and finance, but not yet in SHM-based decision. SHM-based decision-making is particularly susceptible to the representativeness heuristic, where simplified rules for updating probabilities can distort the decision maker’s perception of risk. In this paper, we examine how representativeness affects the interpretation of data, providing a deeper understanding of the differences between a heuristic method affected by cognitive biases and the classical approach. Our study is conducted both theoretically through comparison with formal Bayesian methods as well as empirically through the application to a real-life case study about the evaluation of a bridge safety.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 229.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  1. American Ass. State Highway and Transportation Off. (1997). AASHTO guide for commonly recognized (CoRe) structural elements. AASHTO, Washington D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Autonomous Province of Trento (2018) www.bms.provincia.tn.it/bms

  3. Barberis N, Shleifer A, Vishny R (1998) A model of investor sentiment. J Financ Econ 49(3):307–343

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Bolognani D, Verzobio A, Tonelli D, Cappello C, Glisic B, Zonta D, Quigley J (2018) Quantifying the benefit of structural health monitoring: what if the manager is not the owner? Struct Health Monit 17(6):1393–1409

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Bolognani D, Verzobio A, Tonelli D, Cappello C, Zonta D, Glisic B (2017) An application of prospect theory to a SHM-based decision problem. In: Proceedings of SPIE, Portland

    Google Scholar 

  6. Bolstad WM (2010) Understanding computational Bayesian statistics. Wiley, NJ, USA

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  7. Bordalo P, Coffman K, Gennaioli N, Shleifer A (2016) Stereotypes. Quart J Econ 131(4):1753–1794

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Cappello C, Zonta D, Glisic B (2016) Expected utility theory for monitoring-based decision-making. Proc IEEE 104(8):1647–1661

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Edward W (1968) Conservativism in Human information processing. In: Kleinmuntz B (ed) Formal representation of human judgment. Wiley, New York, pp 17–52

    Google Scholar 

  10. Feigenbaum EA, Feldman J (1963) Computers and thought. McGraw-Hill Inc, New York

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  11. Gigerenzer G (1995) How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instructuion: frequency formats. Psychol Rev 102(4):684–704

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W (2011) Heuristic decision making. Annu Rev Psychol 62:451–482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Gilovich T, Griffin DW, Kahneman D (2002) Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  14. Gong C, Frangopol DM (2020) Condition-based multiobjective maintenance decision making for highway bridges considering risk perceptions. J Struct Eng 146(5)

    Google Scholar 

  15. Grether DM (1980) Bayes rule as a descriptive model: the representativeness heuristic. Quart J Econ 95(3):537–557

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Grether DM (1992) Testing Bayes rule and the representativeness heuristic: some experimental evidence. J Econ Behav Organ 17(1):31–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Kahneman D, Tversky A (1972) Subjective probability: a judgment of representativeness. Cogn Psychol 3:430–454

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Kahneman D, Tversky A (1973) Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cogn Psychol 5(2):207–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Kahneman D, Tversky A (1973) On the psychology of prediction. Psychol Rev 80(4):237–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Kahneman D, Tversky A (1979) Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47(2):263–292

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  21. Melchers RE (1999) Structural reliability: analysis and prediction, 2nd edn. Wiley, Chichester

    Google Scholar 

  22. Neumann JV, Morgenstern O (1944) Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton University Press, New Jersey

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  23. Parmigiani G, Inoue L (2009) Decision theory: principles and approaches. Wiley, Chichester

    Book  Google Scholar 

  24. Raiffa H, Schlaifer R (1961) Applied statistical decision theory. Clinton Press, Boston

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  25. Romanycia MH, Pelletier FJ (1985) What is a heuristic? Comput Intell 1(1):47–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Sivia D, Skilling J (2006) Data analysis: a Bayesian tutorial. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  27. Tenenbaum JB, Griffiths T (2001) The rational basis of representativeness. In: Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science society. 23. Edinburgh: Proceedings of the 23rd annual conference of the cognitive science society

    Google Scholar 

  28. Tonelli D, Verzobio A, Cappello C, Bolognani D, Zonta D, Bursi SO & Costa C (2017) Expected utility theory for monitoring-based decision support system. Stanford, USA, Proceedings of the 11th International Workshop on Structural Health Monitoring

    Google Scholar 

  29. Tonelli D, Verzobio A, Bolognani D, Cappello C, Glisic B, Zonta D, & Quigley J (2018) The conditional value of information of SHM: what if the manager is not the owner?. Proceedings of SPIE, Denver (USA)

    Google Scholar 

  30. Tonge FM (1960) Summary of a heuristic line balancing procedure. Manage Sci 7(1):21–42

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  31. Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Sci New Ser 185:1124–1131

    Google Scholar 

  32. Tversky A, Kahneman D (1983) Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychol Rev 90(4):293–315

    Google Scholar 

  33. Verzobio A, Bolognani D, Quigley J & Zonta D (2018) The consequences of Heuristic distortions on SHM-based decision problems. Manchester, UK, Proceedings of the 9th European Workshop of Structural Health Monitoring

    Google Scholar 

  34. Verzobio A, Bolognani, D, Zonta D & Quigley J (2019) Quantifying the benefit of structural health monitoring: can the value of information be negative?. Proceedings of the 12th International Workshop on Structural Health Monitoring, Stanford (USA)

    Google Scholar 

  35. Verzobio A, El-Awady A, Ponnambalam K, Quigley J & Zonta D (2020, in press): An elicitation process to quantify Bayesian networks for dam failure analysis. Can J Civ Eng. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1139/cjce-2020-0089

  36. Zonta D, Glisic B, Adriaenssesns S (2014) Value of information: impact of monitoring on decision-making. Struct Control Health Monit 21:1043–1056

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Zonta D, Zandonini R, Bortot F (2007) A reliability-based bridge management concept. Struct Infrastruct Eng 3(3):215–235

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrea Verzobio .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this paper

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this paper

Verzobio, A., Bolognani, D., Quigley, J., Zonta, D. (2021). Consequences of Heuristic Distortions on SHM-Based Decision. In: Rainieri, C., Fabbrocino, G., Caterino, N., Ceroni, F., Notarangelo, M.A. (eds) Civil Structural Health Monitoring. CSHM 2021. Lecture Notes in Civil Engineering, vol 156. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-74258-4_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-74258-4_8

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-74257-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-74258-4

  • eBook Packages: EngineeringEngineering (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics